Skip to main content

Abstract

That it is even possible to take a phenomenological approach to child development rests upon a radical empirical fact: To be human means that one is or was once a child. The experience of being or having been a child is known to every person as an essential condition of his or her existence. This knowledge is accessible either directly to children who are currently living through their own developing childhoods or indirectly to adults who recollect childhood experiences out of the fabric of their already developed life-worlds. It is the radicality of this “world fact” (Wild, 1959) concerning childhood that makes possible a phenomenological approach to child development. For one cannot make sense of the world of a developing child without existential grounding in the experience of living or having lived as a child. And one cannot hope to communicate with others about the meaning of the developing child’s world unless those others, too, are or have been children.

And the children in the apple-tree

Not known, because not looked for...

—T. S. Eliot

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 74.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Aries, P. (1962). Centuries of childhood. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, E. (1971). The birth and death of meaning. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Briod, M. (1986). The child’s sense of time and the clock. Phenomenology & Pedagogy 4(1), 9–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coles, R. (1986). The moral life of children. Boston: Atlantic Monthly Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennison, G. (1969). The lives of children. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, M. (1978). Children’s minds. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eliot, T. S. (1943). Four quartets. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and World.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. H. (1980). Identity and the life cycle. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giorgi, A. (1983). Concerning the possibility of phenomenological psychological research. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology 14, 129–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1962). Being and time. London: SCM Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1962). Die krisis der eupaischen Wissenschaften und die transzendentale phanomenologie (2nd ed.). The Hague: M. Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. (1984). The nature of the child. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knowles, R. (1986). Human development and human possibility: Erikson in the light of Heidegger. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koestler, A. (1974). Selections from arrow in the blue. In J. I. Milgram and R. P. Hawkins (Eds.), Childhood revisited (pp. 170–181). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koestler, A. (1978). Janus. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohl, H. (1984). Growing minds. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347–480). Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kotre, J. (1984). Outliving the self. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippitz, W. (1983). The child’s understanding of time. Phenomenology & Pedagogy 2(3), 172–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippitz, W. (1986). Understanding children, communicating with children: Approaches to the child within us, before us, and with us. Phenomenology & Pedagogy 4(3), 56–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, G. B. (1980). Philosophy and the young child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, G. B. (1984). Dialogues with children. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCullough, D. W. (1987). American childhoods: An anthology. Boston: Little, Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception (C. Smith, Trans.). New York: Humanities.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1964). The child’s relations with others (W. Cobb, Trans.). The primacy of perception (pp. 96–155). Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1973). Consciousness and the acquisition of language. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, J. I., and Hawkins, R. P. (Eds.) (1974). Childhood revisited. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. (1981). The drama of the gifted child. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paley, V. B. (1981). Wally’s stories: Conversations in the kindergarten. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pitcher, E. G., and Prelinger, E. (1963). Children tell stories. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polakow-Suransky, V. (1982). The erosion of childhood. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polakow-Suransky, V. (1984). The social landscape of play: The child as researcher. Paper presented to The American Education Research Association in New Orleans, April 23, 1984.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prescott, M. P., & Valle, R. S. (1978). An existential-phenomenological look at cognitive-development theory and research. In R. S. Valle and M. King (Eds.), Existential-phenomenological alternatives for psychology (pp. 153–165). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schrag, C.O. (1980). Radical reflection and the origin of the human sciences. W. Lafayette, IN: Purdue University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton-Smith, B. (1980). Folkstories of children. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Berg, J. H. (1961). The changing nature of man. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wild, J. (1959). Human freedom and the social order. Durham: Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Briod, M. (1989). A Phenomenological Approach to Child Development. In: Valle, R.S., Halling, S. (eds) Existential-Phenomenological Perspectives in Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-6989-3_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-6989-3_7

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-0-306-43044-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-6989-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics