Advertisement

A Cognitive Reinterpretation of Classical Introspectionism

The Relation between Introspection and Altered States of Consciousness and Their Mutual Relevance for a Cognitive Psychology of Metaphor and Felt Meaning
  • Harry T. Hunt
Chapter
Part of the Annals of Theoretical Psychology book series (AOTP, volume 4)

Abstract

This paper is a step toward relinking the scattered subjectivist-phenomenological traditions in psychology to offer some balance for our current “extraverted” obsession with the experimental management of information and computation. Felt meaning, physiognomy, and metaphor are crucial to such a Fechnerian endeavor, because these most subjective aspects of symbolic cognition are also most strikingly attuned to the qualia afforded by the environment.

Specifically, the striking overlap between the techniques and phenomena of altered states of consciousness and classical introspectionism, understood through a holistic cognitive perspective, exteriorizes normally masked aspects of metaphor—the synaesthetically mediated reorganization of microgenetic-iconic stages of perception. Titchener’s sensory-affective core has the place in a psychology of metaphor that it so lacked in functional perception (Gibson), and the disparity between Würzburg “impalpables” of thought and Cornell imagery protocols can be resolved if we assume that all representational processes have a presentational aspect based on complex or geometric synesthesias. Support for this approach is drawn from Wittgenstein’s later notebooks.

Keywords

Altered State Body Percept Memory Color Psychedelic Drug Semantic Satiation 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ach, N. (1951). Determining tendencies; awareness. In D. Rapaport (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought (pp. 15–38). New York: Columbia University Press. (Original work published 1905)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adams, G. (1923). An experimental study of memory color and related phenomena. American Journal of Psychology, 23, 359–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Angyal, A. (1935). The perceptual basis of somatic delusions in a case of schizophrenia. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 34, 270–279.Google Scholar
  4. Angyal, A. (1936). The experience of the body-self in schizophrenia. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 35, 1029–1053.Google Scholar
  5. Angyal, A. (1937). Disturbances of activity in a case of schizophrenia. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 38, 1047–1054.Google Scholar
  6. Arnheim, R. (1969). Visual thinking. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  7. Arnheim, R. (1974). Art and visual perception. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  8. Bartlett, F. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Bion, W. R. (1965). Transformations. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  10. Boisen, A. (1962). The exploration of the inner world. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
  11. Brentano, F. (1960). The distinction between mental and physical phenomena. In R. M. Chisholm (Ed.), Realism and the background of phenomenology (pp. 39–61). Glencoe, IL: Free Press. (Original work published 1874)Google Scholar
  12. Brown, D., Forte, M., & Dysart, M. (1984). Visual sensitivity and mindfulness meditation. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 58, 775–784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bühler, K. (1951). On thought connections. In D. Rapaport (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought (pp. 39–57). New York: Columbia University Press. (Original work published 1908)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Burnett, N., & Dallenbach, K. (1927). The experience of heat. American Journal of Psychology, 38, 418–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cassirer, E. (1955). The philosophy of symbolic forms (Vol. 2). New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Cattell, R. (1930). The subjective character of cognition and the presentational development of perception. British Journal of Psychology Monographs, 14, 1–166.Google Scholar
  17. Celine, L. (1968). Castle to castle. New York: Delacorte.Google Scholar
  18. Chang, G. (Ed.). (1963). Teachings of Tibetan yoga. New York: University Books.Google Scholar
  19. Chapman, J. (1966). The early symptoms of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 112, 225–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Clarke, H. M. (1911). Conscious attitudes. American Journal of Psychology, 22, 214–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Deikman, A. (1971). Bimodal consciousness. Archives of General Psychiatry, 25, 481–489.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Dennett, D. (1978). Toward a cognitive theory of consciousness. In C. Savage (Ed.), Perception and cognition issues in the foundations of psychology. Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science (pp. 201–228). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
  23. Don, W., & Weld, H. (1924). Lapse of meaning with visual fixation. American Journal of Psychology, 35, 446–453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Duncker, K. (1947). Phenomenology and epistemology of consciousness of objects. Philosophical and Phenomenological Research, 7, 505–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Edelson, M. (1975). Language and interpretation in psychoanalysis. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Evans, R. (1972). E. B. Titchener and his lost system. Journal of the History of Behavioural Sciences, 8, 168–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Fischer, R. (1975). Cartography of inner space. In R. Siegel & L. West (Eds.), Hallucinations: Behavior, experience, and theory (pp. 197–239). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  28. Flavell, J., & Draguns, J. (1957). A microgenetic approach to perception and thought. Psychological Bulletin, 54, 197–217.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Foucault, M. (1978). The history of sexuality. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
  30. Freud, S. (1957). The unconscious. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 14, pp. 159–215). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1915)Google Scholar
  31. Freud, S. (1959). The “uncanny.” In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 17, pp. 217–256). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1919)Google Scholar
  32. Gendlin, E. (1962). Experiencing and the creation of meaning. New York: Free Press of Glencoe.Google Scholar
  33. Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Brain, 88, 237–294, 585–644.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Gibson, J. (1963). The useful dimensions of sensitivity. American Psychologist, 18, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Gibson, J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  36. Globus, G. G. (1973). Consciousness and brain: II. Introspection, the qualia of experience, and the unconscious. Archives of General Psychiatry, 29, 167–176.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  38. Goleman, D. (1972). The Buddha on meditation and states of consciousness. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 4, 1–44.Google Scholar
  39. Govinda, A. (1960). Foundations of Tibetan mysticism. New York: Dutton.Google Scholar
  40. Haber, R. N. (1983). The impending demise of the icon. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 6, 1–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Haskell, R. (1984). Empirical structures of mind: Cognition, linguistics, and transformation. Journal of Mind & Behavior, 5, 29–48.Google Scholar
  42. Hebb, D., & Thompson, W. (1968). The social significance of animal studies. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 729–774). Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  43. Heidegger, M. (1971). On the way to language. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  44. Heidegger, M. (1977). The question concerning technology and other essays. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
  45. Hillman, J. (1978). The therapeutic value of alchemical language. Dragonflies, pp. 33–42.Google Scholar
  46. Hofstadter, D. (1979). Gödel, Escher, Bach: An eternal golden braid. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  47. Humphrey, G. (1951). Thinking. New York: Methuen.Google Scholar
  48. Hunt, H. T. (1984). A cognitive psychology of mystical and altered-state experience. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 58, 467–513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Hunt, H. T. (1985). Cognition and states of consciousness: The necessity for empirical study of ordinary and nonordinary consciousness for contemporary cognitive psychology. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 60, 239–282.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Hunt, H. T. (n.d.). [Research protocol]. Unpublished data.Google Scholar
  51. Hunt, H. T., & Chefurka, C. M. (1976). A test of the psychedelic model of consciousness. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33, 867–896.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Hunt, W. A. (1933). The meaning of pleasantness and unpleasantness. American Journal of Psychology, 45, 345–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Husserl, E. (1970). The crisis of European sciences and transcendental phenomenology. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Jacobson, E. (1911). On meaning and understanding. American Journal of Psychology, 22, 553–577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vols. 1 & 2). New York: Dover.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. James, W. (1912). Does consciousness exist? In W. James, Essays in radical empiricism (pp. 1–38). New York: Longmans, Green. (Original work published 1904)Google Scholar
  57. Jung, C. G. (1953). Psychology and alchemy. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
  58. Jung, C. G. (1959). A study in the process of individuation. In C. G. Jung, The archetypes and the collective unconscious (pp. 290–354). New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
  59. Kakise, H. (1911). A preliminary experimental study of the conscious concomitants of understanding. American Journal of Psychology, 22, 14–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Katz, D. (1935). The world of colour. London: Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  61. Klüver, H. (1966). Mescal and mechanisms of hallucination. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  62. Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.Google Scholar
  63. Krueger, F. (1928). The essence of feeling: Outline of a systematic theory. In M. Reymert (Ed.), Feelings and emotions (pp. 58–85). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
  64. Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  65. Laski, M. (1961). Ecstasy. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  66. Levi-Strauss, C. (1966). The savage mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  67. Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  69. Marcel, A. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 238–300.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Marks, L. E. (1978). The unity of the senses. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  71. Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  72. Milner, M. (1957). On not being able to paint. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  73. Minkowski, E. (1970). Lived time. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
  74. Nafe, J. P. (1924). An experimental study of the affective qualities. American Journal of Psychology, 35, 507–544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Natsoulas, T. (1981). Basic problems of consciousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 132–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.Google Scholar
  77. Otto, R. (1958). The idea of the holy (J. Harvey, Trans.). New York: Galaxy Books. (Original work published 1923)Google Scholar
  78. Perky, C. W. (1910). An experimental study of imagination. American Journal of Psychology, 21, 422–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Rahn, C. (1913). The relation of sensation to other categories in contemporary psychology: A study in the psychology of thinking. Psychological Review Monographs, 67, 1–131.Google Scholar
  80. Rahn, C. (1928). Science and the religious life: A psycho-physiological approach. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  81. Reich, W. (1960). Selected writings. New York: Noonday Press.Google Scholar
  82. Ricoeur, P. (1977). The rule of metaphor. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
  83. Rycroft, C. (1979). The innocence of dreams. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
  84. Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. New York: Barnes & Noble.Google Scholar
  85. Sampson, E. (1981). Cognitive psychology as ideology. American Psychologist, 36,730–743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Sander, F. (1930). Structures, totality of experience, and gestalt. In C. Murchison (Ed.), Psychologies of 1930 (pp. 188–204). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Sartre, J. P. (1947). A fundamental idea of the phenomenology of Husserl: Intentionality. In J. P. Sartre, Situations I. Paris: Gallinard. (Original work published 1939, J. Mayer, Trans.)Google Scholar
  88. Schilder, P. (1933). Experiments on imagination, after images, and hallucinations. American Journal of Psychiatry, 13, 597–611.Google Scholar
  89. Schilder, P. (1942). Mind: Perception and thought in their constructive aspects. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  90. Schutz, A. (1962). On multiple realities. In A. Schutz, The problems of social reality: Collected papers of Alfred Schutz (Vol. 1, pp. 207–259). The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  91. Searle, J. (1980). Minds, brains, programs. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 3, 427–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Severance, E., & Washburn, M. (1907). The loss of associative power in words after long fixation. American Journal of Psychology, 18, 182–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Shattock, E. (1960). An experiment in mindfulness. New York: Dutton.Google Scholar
  94. Smith, D., & Raygor, A. (1956). Verbal satiation and personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 323–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Smith, G. (1957). Visual perception: An event over time. Psychological Review, 64, 306–313.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Spearman, C. (1923). The nature of intelligence and the principles of cognition. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  97. Swartz, P. (1983). Concerning the continuous present. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 57,159–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Titchener, E. B. (1909). Lectures on the experimental psychology of the thought processes. New York: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Titchener, E. B. (1912). Description vs. statement of meaning. American Journal of Psychology, 23, 165–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Titchener, E. B. (1915). Sensation and system. American Journal of Psychology, 26, 258–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Titchener, E. B. (1929). Systematic psychology: Prolegomena. New York: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. VanDusen, W. (1972). The natural depth in man. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  103. Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language (E. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1934)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Werner, H. (1956). Microgenesis and aphasia. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 347–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Werner, H. (1961). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: Science Education.Google Scholar
  106. Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  107. Winnicott, D. W. (1971). Playing and reality. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  108. Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  109. Wittgenstein, L. (1966). Lectures and conversations on aesthetics, psychology and religious belief Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  110. Wittgenstein, L. (1980). Culture and value. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  111. Wittgenstein, L. (1980). Remarks on the philosophy of psychology (Vols. 1 & 2). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  112. Wittgenstein, L. (1982). Last writings on the philosophy of psychology (Vol. 1). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  113. Woodworth, R. S. (1906). Imageless thought. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Method, 3, 701–708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Young, P. (1927). Studies in affective psychology. American Journal of Psychology, 38, 157–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Zener, K., & Gaffron, M. (1962). Perceptual experience: An analysis of its relation to the external world through internal processings. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 4, pp. 516–618). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • Harry T. Hunt
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyBrock UniversitySt. CatharinesCanada

Personalised recommendations