Individual Voice Discrimination in Birds

  • Marcel M. Lambrechts
  • Andre A. Dhondt
Part of the Current Ornithology book series (CUOR, volume 12)

Abstract

Animals discriminate among familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics probably to avoid wasting time and energy during social interactions. Individual recognition may reduce the chance of expensive fights among territorial neighbors during resource defense, or allow mates, kin, or parents and offspring to find each other where familiar individuals intermingle with unfamiliar ones. Because individual recognition may involve a time-consuming learning process, or because it may be costly in some other way, individual recognition will probably only evolve when benefits outweigh these costs (e.g., Beer, 1970; Wilson, 1975; Falls, 1982, Beecher and Stoddard, 1990).

Keywords

Dura Defend Indigo Congo Larg 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Baker, M. C., Thompson, D. B., and Sherman, G. L., 1981, Neighbor/stranger song discrimination in the White-crowned Sparrow, Condor 83:265–267.Google Scholar
  2. Baker, M. C., Bjerke, T. K., Lampe, H., and Espmark, Y., 1986, Sexual response of female Great Tits to variation in sizes of males’ song repertoires, Am. Nat. 128:491–498.Google Scholar
  3. Baptista, L. F., 1985, The functional significance of song sharing in the White-crowned Sparrow, Can. J. Zool. 63:1741–1752.Google Scholar
  4. Bauer, H.-G., and Nagl, W., 1992, Individual distinctiveness and possible function of song parts of Short-toed Treeceepers (Certhia brachydactyla). Evidence from multivariate song analysis, Ethology 91:108–121.Google Scholar
  5. Becker, P. H., 1982, The coding of species-specific characteristics in bird sounds, in: Acoustic Communication in Birds, Volume 2 (D. E. Kroodsma, and E. H. Miller, eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 213–252.Google Scholar
  6. Beecher, M. D., 1982, Signature systems and kin recognition, Amer. Zool. 22:477–490.Google Scholar
  7. Beecher, M. D., 1989, Signalling systems for individual recognition: an information theory approach, Anim. Behav. 38:248–261.Google Scholar
  8. Beecher, M. D., 1990, The evolution of parent-offspring recognition in swallows, in: Contemporary issues in Comparative Psychology (D. A. Dewsbury, ed.), Sinauer, Mass., pp. 360–Google Scholar
  9. Beecher, M. D., and Stoddard, P. K., 1990, The role of bird song and calls in individual recognition: contrasting field and laboratory perspectives, in: Comparative Perception, Vol. 2 (W. C. Stebbins, and M. A. Berkeley, eds.), New York, John Wiley, pp. 375–408.Google Scholar
  10. Beecher, M. D., Stoddard, P. K., and Loesche, P., 1985, Recognition of parents’ voices by young Cliff Swallows, Auk 102:600–605.Google Scholar
  11. Beecher, M. D., Medvin, M. B., Stoddard, P. K., and Loesche, P., 1986, Acoustic adaptations for parent-offspring recognition in Swallows, Exp. Biol. 45:179–193.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Beecher, M. D., Campbell, S. E., and Burt, J. M., 1994a, Song perception in the Song Sparrow: birds classify by song type but not by singer, Anim. Behav. 47:1343–1351.Google Scholar
  13. Beecher, M. D., Campbell, S. E., and Stoddard, P. K., 1994b, Correlation of song learning and territory establishment strategies in the Song Sparrow, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci., U.S.A., 91:1450–1454.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Beer, C. G., 1970, Individual recognition of voice in the social behavior of birds, Adv. Study Behav. 3:27–74.Google Scholar
  15. Belcher, J. W., and Thompson, W. L., 1969, Territorial defense and individual song recognition in the Indigo Bunting, lack-Pine Warbler 47:76–83.Google Scholar
  16. Beletsky, L. D., 1983, An investigation of individual recognition by voice in female Redwinged Blackbirds, Anim. Behav. 31:355–362.Google Scholar
  17. Borror, D. J., 1959, Variation in the songs of the Rufous-sided Towhee, Wilson Bull. 71:54–72.Google Scholar
  18. Brémond, J. C., 1968, Recherches sur la sémantique et les éléments vecteurs d’information dans les signaux acoustiques du Rouge-Gorge (Erithacus rubecula L.), Terre Vie 2:109–220.Google Scholar
  19. Brémond, J. C., Aubin, T., Nyamsi, R. M., and Robisson, P., 1990, Le chant du Manchot Empereur (Aptenodytes forsteri): Recherche des paramètres utilisables pour la reconnaissance individuelle. C.R. Acad. Sci. Ser. III Sci. Vie 311:31–35.Google Scholar
  20. Brindley, E. L., 1991, Response of European Robins to playback of song: neighbour recognition and overlapping, Anim. Behav. 41:503–512.Google Scholar
  21. Brooks, R. J., and Falls, J. B., 1975, Individual recognition by song in White-throated Sparrows: III. Song features used in individual recognition, Can. J. Zool. 53:1749–1761.Google Scholar
  22. Chaiken, M., 1992, Individual recognition of nestling distress screams by European Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris), Behaviour 120:138–150.Google Scholar
  23. Craig, J. L., and Jenkins, P. F., 1982, The evolution of complexity in broadcast song in passerines, J. Theor. Biol. 95:415–422.Google Scholar
  24. Cynx, J., Williams, H., and Nottebohm, F., 1990, Timbre discrimination in Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata) song syllables, J. Comp. Psychol. 104:303–308.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Dhondt, A. A., and Lambrechts, M. M., 1992, Individual voice recognition in birds, TREE 7:178–179.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Dooling, R. J., 1980, Behavior and psychophysics of hearing in birds, in: Comparative Studies of Hearing in Vertebrates, (A. N. Popper, and R. R. Fay, eds.), Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp. 261–288Google Scholar
  27. Eens, M., Pinxten, R., Verheyen, R. F., 1992, Song learning in captive European Starlings, Sturnus vulgaris, Anim. Behav. 44:1131–1143.Google Scholar
  28. Elfström, S. T., 1990, Individual and species-specific song patterns of Rock and Meadow Pipits: physical characteristics and experiments, Bioacoustics 2:277–301.Google Scholar
  29. Emlen, S. T., 1971, The role of song in individual recognition in the Indigo Bunting, Z. Tierpsychol. 28:241–246.Google Scholar
  30. Falls, J. B., 1982, Individual recognition by sounds in birds, in: Acoustic Communication in Birds, Volume 2 (D. E. Kroodsma, and E. H. Miller, eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 237–278.Google Scholar
  31. Falls, J. B., 1992, Playback: a historical perspective, in: Playback and Studies of Animal Communication (P. K. McGregor, ed.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 11–33.Google Scholar
  32. Falls, J. B., and Brooks, R. J., 1975, Individual recognition by song in White-throated Sparrows: II. Effects of location, Can. J. Zool. 53:1412–1420.Google Scholar
  33. Falls, J. B., and d’Agincourt, L. G., 1981, A comparison of neighbor-stranger discrimination in Eastern and Western Meadowlarks, Can. J. Zool. 59:2380–2385.Google Scholar
  34. Falls, J. B., Krebs, J. R., and McGregor, P. K., 1982, Song matching in the great tits (Parus major): the effects of similarity and familiarity, Anim. Behav. 30:997–1009.Google Scholar
  35. Galeotti, P., and Pavan, G., 1991, Individual recognition of male Towny Owls (Strix aluco) using spectrograms of their territorial calls, Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 3:113–126.Google Scholar
  36. Galeotti, P., and Pavan, G., 1993, Differential responses of territorial Tawny Owls Strix aluco to the hooting of neighbours and strangers, Ibis 135:300–304.Google Scholar
  37. Getty, T., 1989, Are dear enemies in a war of attrition?, Anim. Behav. 37:337–339.Google Scholar
  38. Giraldeau, L.-A., and Ydenberg, R., 1987, The center-edge effect: the result of a war of attrition between territorial contestants, Auk 104:535–538.Google Scholar
  39. Godard, R., 1991, Long-term memory of individual neighbours in a migratory songbird, Nature 350:228–229.Google Scholar
  40. Godard, R., 1993, Tit for tat among neighboring Hooded Warblers, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 33:45–50.Google Scholar
  41. Goldman, P., 1973, Song recognition by Field Sparrows, Auk 90:106–113.Google Scholar
  42. Hansen, P., 1981, Coordinated singing in neighbouring Yellowhammers (Emberiza citrinella), Natura Jutlandica 19:121–138.Google Scholar
  43. Hansen, P., 1984, Neighbour-stranger song discrimination in territorial Yellowhammer Emberiza citrinella males, and a comparison with responses to own and alien song dialects, Ornis scandinavia 15:240–247.Google Scholar
  44. Harris, M. A., and Lemon, R. E., 1972, Songs of Song Sparrows (Melospiza melodia): individual variation and dialects, Can. J. Zool. 50:301–309.Google Scholar
  45. Harris, M. A., and Lemon, R. E., 1976, Response of male Song Sparrows Melospiza melodia to neighbouring and non-neighbouring individuals, Ibis 118:421–424.Google Scholar
  46. Helb, H.-W., Dowsett-Lemaire, F., Bergmann, H.-H., and Conrads, K., 1985, Mixed singing in European songbirds—a review, Z. Tierpsychol. 69:27–41.Google Scholar
  47. Homing, C. L., Beecher, M. D., Stoddard, P. K., and Campbell, S. E., 1993, Song perception in the Song Sparrow: Importance of different parts of the song in song type classification, Ethology 94:46–58.Google Scholar
  48. Järvi, T., Radesäter, T., and Jakobsson, S., 1977, Individual recognition and variation in the Great Tit (Parus major) two-syllable song. Biophon 5:4–9.Google Scholar
  49. Jouventin, P., 1982, Visual and vocal signals in Penguins, their evolution and adaptive characters, Berlin, Verlag Paul Parey.Google Scholar
  50. Kent, J. P., 1992, The relationship between the hen and chick, Gallus gallus domesticus: the hen’s recognition of the chick, Anim. Behav. 44:996–998.Google Scholar
  51. Kersta, L. G., 1962, Voiceprint identification, Nature 186:1253–1257.Google Scholar
  52. Krebs, J. R., 1971, Territory and breeding density in the Great Tit, Parus major L., Ecology 52:2–22.Google Scholar
  53. Krebs, J. R., 1976, Habituation and song repertoires in the Great Tit, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1:215–227.Google Scholar
  54. Krebs, J. R., 1977, The significance of song repertoires, the Beau Geste hypothesis, Anim. Behav. 25:475–478.Google Scholar
  55. Krebs, J. R., and Kroodsma, D. E., 1980, Repertoires and geographical variation in bird song, Adv. in Study Behav. 11:143–177.Google Scholar
  56. Kroodsma, D. E., 1976, The effect of large song repertoires on neighbor ‘recognition’ in male song sparrows, Condor 78:97–99.Google Scholar
  57. Kroodsma, D. E., 1982, Song repertoires: Problems in their definition and use, in: Evolution and Ecology of Acoustic Communication in Birds, Vol. 2, (D. E. Kroodsma, and E. H. Miller, eds.), Academic Press, London, pp. 125–146.Google Scholar
  58. Kroodsma, D. E., 1989, Suggested experimental designs for song playbacks, Anim. Behav. 37:600–609.Google Scholar
  59. Kroodsma, D. E., and Byers, B. E., 1991, The function(s) of bird song, Amer. Zool. 31:318–328.Google Scholar
  60. Kroodsma, D. E., and Canady, R. A., 1985, Differences in repertoire size, singing behavior, and associated neuroatonomy among Marsh Wren populations have a genetic basis, Auk 102:439–446.Google Scholar
  61. Kroodsma, D. E., and Miller, E. H., 1982, Acoustic Communication in Birds, Volume 1 and 2, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  62. Lambrechts, M. M., 1992, Male quality and playback in the Great Tit, in: Playback and Studies of Animal Communication (P. K. McGregor, ed.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 135–152.Google Scholar
  63. Lambrechts, M., and Dhondt, A. A., 1987, Differences in singing performance between male great tits, Ardea 75:43–52.Google Scholar
  64. Lampe, H. M., and Slagsvold, T., 1994, Individual recognition based on male song in a female songbird, J. Ornithol. 135:163.Google Scholar
  65. Lemon, R. E., 1975, How birds develop song dialects, Condor 77:385–406.Google Scholar
  66. Lemon, R. E., Cotter, R., MacNally, R. C., Monette, S., 1985, Song repertoires and song sharing by American Redstarts, Condor 87:457–470.Google Scholar
  67. Loesche, P., Beecher, M. D., and Stoddard, P. K., 1992, Perception of Cliff Swallow calls by birds and humans, J. Comp. Psychol. 106:239–247.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Loesche, P., Stoddard, P. K., Higgins, B. J., and Beecher, M. D., 1991, Signature versus perceptual adaptations for individual vocal recognition in Swallows, Behaviour 118:15–25.Google Scholar
  69. McArthur, P. D., 1982, Mechanisms and development of parent-young recognition in the Pinon Jay (Bymnorhinus cyanocephalus), Anim. Behav. 30:62–7Google Scholar
  70. McGregor, P. K., 1989, Pro-active memory interference in neighbour recognition by a song bird. Proc. Int. Orn. Cong. XIX:1391–1397.Google Scholar
  71. McGregor, P. K., 1991, The singer and the song: on the receiving end of bird song, Biol. Rev. 66:57–81.Google Scholar
  72. McGregor, P. K., 1993, Signalling in territorial systems: a context for individual identification, ranging and eavesdropping, Phil. Trans. B. Soc. Lond. B 340:237–244.Google Scholar
  73. McGregor, P. K., and Avery, M. I., 1986, The unsung songs of Great Tits (Parus major): learning neighbours’ songs for discrimination, Behav. Ecol. Sociohiol. 18:311–316.Google Scholar
  74. McGregor, P. K., and Krebs, J. R., 1982, Song types in a population of Great Tits (Parus major): their distribution, abundance, and acquisition by individuals, Behaviour 79:126–152.Google Scholar
  75. McGregor, P. K., and Krebs, J. R., 1984a, Song learning and deceptive mimicry, Anim. Behav. 32:280–287.Google Scholar
  76. McGregor, P. K., and Krebs, J. R., 1984b, Sound degradation as a distance cue in great tit (Parus major) song, Behav. Ecol. Sociohiol. 16:49–56.Google Scholar
  77. McGregor, P. K., Catchpole, C. K., Dabelsteen, T., Falls, J. B., Fusani, L., Gerhardt, H. C., Gilbert, F., Horn, A. G., Klump, G. M., Kroodsma, D. E., Lambrechts, M. M., McComb, K. E., Nelson, D. A., Pepperberg, I. M., Ratcliffe, L., Searcy, W. A., and Weary, D. M., 1992, Design of playback experiments: the thornbridge hall NATO ARW consensus, in: Playback and Studies of Animal Communication, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 1–9.Google Scholar
  78. Marler, P., 1960, Bird song and mate selection, in: Animal Sounds and Communication, (W. F. Lanyon, and W. Tavolga, eds.), A.I.B.S., Washington, D.C., pp. 348–367Google Scholar
  79. Martin, D. J., 1990, Songs of the Fox Sparrow. III. Ordering of songs, Wilson Bull. 102:655–671.Google Scholar
  80. Medvin, M. B., and Beecher, M. D., 1986, Parent-offspring recognition in the Barn Swallow, Anim. Behav. 34:1627–1639.Google Scholar
  81. Morton, E. S., 1982, Grading, discreteness, redundancy and motivation-structure rules, in: Acoustic Communication in Birds, Volume 1 (D. E. Kroodsma, and E. H. Miller, eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 183–212.Google Scholar
  82. Nelson, D. A., 1989, Song frequency as a cue for recognition of species and individuals in the Field Sparrow (Spizella pusilla), J. Comp. Psychol. 103:171–176.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Nottebohm, F., Kasparian, S., and Pandazis, C., 1981, Brain space for a learned task, Brain Research 213:99–109.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Payne, R. B., 1981, Song learning and social interaction in Indigo Buntings. Anim. Behav. 29:688–697.Google Scholar
  85. Payne, R. B., 1982, Ecological consequences of song matching: breeding success and intraspecific song mimicry in Indigo Buntings, Ecology 63:401–411.Google Scholar
  86. Payne, R. B., 1983, The social context of song mimicry: song-matching dialects in Indigo Buntings (Passerina cyanea), Anim. Behav. 31:788–805.Google Scholar
  87. Payne, R. B., Payne, L. L., and Rowley, I., 1988, Kin and social relationships in Splendid Fairy-wrens: recognition by song in a cooperative bird, Anim. Behav. 36:1341–1351.Google Scholar
  88. Payne, R. B., Payne, L. L., Rowley, I., and Russell, E. M., 1991, Social recognition and response to song in cooperative Red-winged Fairy-wrens, Auk 108:811–819.Google Scholar
  89. Petrinovich, L., and Peeke, H. V. S., 1973, Habituation to territorial song in the White-crowned Sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys), Behav. Biol. 8:743–748.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  90. Pickstock, J. C., and Krebs, J. R., 1980, Neighbour-stranger discrimination in the Chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs), J. Ornithol. 121:105–108.Google Scholar
  91. Podos, J., Peters, S., Rudnicky, T., Marler, P., and Nowicki, S., 1992, The organization of song repertoires in song sparrows: themes and variations, Ethology 90:89–106.Google Scholar
  92. Ratcliffe, L. M., 1990, Neighbour/stranger discrimination of whistled songs in Black-capped Chickadees, Proc. Int. Orn. Congr. XX (Abstract 852).Google Scholar
  93. Ratcliffe, L., and Weisman, R., 1992, Pitch processing strategies in birds: a comparison of laboratory and field studies, in: Playback and Studies of Animal Communication, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 211–223.Google Scholar
  94. Richards, D. G., 1979, Recognition of neighbors by associative learning in Rufous-sided Towhees, Auk 96:688–693.Google Scholar
  95. Richards, D. G., 1981, Estimation of distance of singing conspecifics by the Carolina Wren, Auk 98:127–133.Google Scholar
  96. Ritchison, G., 1988, Responses of Yellow-breasted Chats to the song of neighboring and non-neighboring conspecifics, J. Field Ornithol. 59:37–42.Google Scholar
  97. Robisson, P., 1990, The importance of the temporal pattern of syllables and the syllable structure of display calls for individual recognition in the genus Aptenodytes, Behav. Processes 22:157–163.Google Scholar
  98. Robisson, P., 1992, Vocalizations in Aptenodytes Penguins: Application of the two-voice theory, Auk 109:654–658.Google Scholar
  99. Ryan, M. J., and Brenowitz, E. A., 1985, The role of body size, phylogeny, and ambient noise in the evolution of bird song, Am. Nat. 126:87–100.Google Scholar
  100. Saunders, D. A., and Wooller, R. D., 1988, Consistent individuality of voice in birds as a management tool, Emu 88:25–32.Google Scholar
  101. Schleidt, W. M., 1976, On individuality: the constituents of distinctiveness, in: Perspectives in Ethology, Vol. 2 (P. P. G. Bateson, and P. H. Klopfer, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 299–310.Google Scholar
  102. Schroeder, D. J., and Wiley, R. H., 1983, Communication with shared song themes in Tufted Titmice, Auk 100:414–424.Google Scholar
  103. Searcy, W. A., McArthur, P. D., Peters, S. S., and Marler, P., 1981, Response of male song and Swamp Sparrows to neighbour, stranger and self songs, Behaviour 77:152–166.Google Scholar
  104. Searcy, W. A., Coffman, S., and Raikow, D. F., 1994, Habituation, recovery, and the similarity of song types within song repertoires in Red-winged Blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus) (Aves, Emberizidae), Ethology 98:38–49.Google Scholar
  105. Shy, E., McGregor, P. K., and Krebs, J. R., 1986, Discrimination of song types by male Great Tits, Behav. Proc. 13:1–12.Google Scholar
  106. Sieber, O. J., 1985, Individual recognition of parental calls by Bank Swallow chicks (Riparia riparia), Anim. Behav. 33:107–116.Google Scholar
  107. Slater, P. J. B., 1989, Bird song learning: causes and consequences, Ethology, Ecology and Evolution 1:19–46.Google Scholar
  108. Slater, P. J., 1991, The response of the Silvereyes to individual variation in their territorial song, Emu 91:36–40.Google Scholar
  109. Speirs, E. A. H., and Davis, L. S., 1991, Discrimination by Adelie Penguins, Pygoscelis adeliae, between the loud mutual calls of mates, neighbours and strangers, Anim. Behav. 41:937–944.Google Scholar
  110. Stoddard, P. K., and Beecher, M. D., 1983, Parental recognition of offspring in the Cliff Swallow, Auk 100:795–799.Google Scholar
  111. Stoddard, P. K., Beecher, M. D., and Willis, M. S., 1988, Response of territorial male Song Sparrows to song types and variations, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 22:125–130.Google Scholar
  112. Stoddard, P. K., Beecher, M. D., Horning, C. L., and Willis, M. S., 1990, Strong neighbor-stranger discrimination in Song Sparrows, Condor 92:1051–1056.Google Scholar
  113. Stoddard, P. K., Beecher, M. D., Horning, C. L., and Campbell, S. E., 1991, Recognition of individual neighbors by song in the Song Sparrow, a species with song repertoires, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 29:211–215.Google Scholar
  114. Stoddard, P. K., Beecher, M. D., Campbell, S. E., and Horning, C. L., 1992, Song-type matching in the Song Sparrow, Can. J. Zool. 70:1440–1444.Google Scholar
  115. Stoddard, P. K., Beecher, M. D., Loeshe, P., and Campbell, S. E., 1992, Does memory constrain individual recognition in a bird with song repertoires? Behaviour 122:274–287.Google Scholar
  116. Tosi, O., 1979, Voice identification. Theory and legal applications, University Park Press, Baltimore.Google Scholar
  117. Van Elsacker, L., Pinxten, R., and Verheyen, R., 1988, Timing of offspring recognition in adult Starlings, Behaviour 107:122–130.Google Scholar
  118. Verner, J., and Milligan, M. M., 1971, Responses of male White-crowned Sparrows to playback of recorded songs, Condor 73:56–64.Google Scholar
  119. Weary, D. M., 1990, Categorization of song notes in Great Tits: which acoustic features are used and why?, Anim. Behav. 39:450–457.Google Scholar
  120. Weary, D. M., and Krebs, J. R., 1992, Great Tits classify songs by individual voice characteristics, Anim. Behav. 43:283–287.Google Scholar
  121. Weary, D. M., Lemon, R. E., and Date, E. M., 1987, Neighbour-stranger discrimination by song in the Veery, a species with song repertoires, Can. J. Zool. 65:1206–1209.Google Scholar
  122. Weary, D. M., Norris, K. J., and Falls, J. B., 1990, Song features birds use to identify individuals, Auk 107:623–625.Google Scholar
  123. Weary, D. M., Lemon, R. E., and Perreault, S., 1992, Song repertoires do not hinder neighbor-stranger recognition, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 31:441–447.Google Scholar
  124. Weeden, J. S., and Falls, J. B., 1959, Differential response of male Ovenbirds to recorded songs of neighboring and more distant individuals, Auk 76:343–351.Google Scholar
  125. Wiley, R. H., and Richards, D. G., 1982, Adaptations for acoustic communication in birds: sound transmission and signal detection, in: Evolution and Ecology of Acoustic Communication in Birds, Vol. 1, (D. E.Kroodsma, and E. H.Miller, eds.), Academic Press,London, pp. 131–181Google Scholar
  126. Wiley, R. H., and Wiley, M. S., 1977, Recognition of neighbors’ duets by Stripe-backed Wrens, Campylorhynchus nuchalis, Behaviour 62:10–34.Google Scholar
  127. Wilson, E. O., 1975, Sociobiology: The New Synthesis, Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  128. Wunderly, J. M., Jr., 1978, Differential response of territorial Yellowthroats to the songs of neighbors and non-neighbors, Auk 95:389–395.Google Scholar
  129. Yasukawa, K., Bick, E., Wagman, D. W, and Marler, P., 1982, Playback and speaker replacements on song-based neighbor, stranger and self-discrimination in male Red-winged Blackbirds, Behav. Ecol. Sociohiol. 10:211–215.Google Scholar
  130. Ydenberg, R. D., Giraldeau, L. A., and Falls, J. B., 1988, Neighbours, strangers and the asymmetric war of attrition. Anim. Behav. 36:343–347.Google Scholar
  131. Zemlin, W. R., 1988, Speech and Hearing Science, Prentice-Hall, Inc., New Jersey.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marcel M. Lambrechts
    • 1
  • Andre A. Dhondt
    • 2
  1. 1.CNRS-CEFEMontpellier Cedex 1France
  2. 2.Cornell Laboratory of OrnithologyIthacaUSA

Personalised recommendations