Advertisement

The Organization of Sequential Behavior

  • Stephen B. Fountain
  • Douglas G. Wallace
  • James D. Rowan

Abstract

For decades, beginning with Lashley’s (1951) celebrated paper or even earlier with the work of Ebbinghaus and others (Hunter, 1920; Skinner, 1934), sequential learning has repeatedly aroused debate over the fundamental nature of learning, memory, and representation in humans and animals. In recent years, debate has centered on whether nonhuman animals can use nonassociative symbolic processes such as rule-induction to learn about the structure of patterned sequences (“serial patterns”) of events. The work by Hulse and colleagues on rats’ serial-pattern learning (SPL) of food reward magnitude in runways, in particular, supported a Rule-Learning (RL) theory (e.g., Hulse & Campbell, 1975; Hulse & Dorsky, 1977; Hulse, 1978; Hulse & Dorsky, 1979; Fountain & Hulse, 1981; Fountain, Evensen, & Hulse, 1983). The RL theory proposed that rats learned some representation of the abstract rules that described organized sequences (Fountain, 1986; Roitblat, Pologe, & Scopatz, 1983; Wathen & Roberts, 1994). The implication was that rats did not have to rely on chaining or remote associations alone to master sequences. Later work in our laboratory involved a somewhat different SPL paradigm employing patterns of stimuli drawn from another stimulus dimension (viz., a spatial dimension rather than food reward magnitude). This work also supported the RL view of serial-pattern learning in both rats and mice (e.g., Fountain, 1990; Fountain & Rowan, 1995b; Fountain & Rowan, 1995a).

Keywords

Serial Position Animal Behavior Process Food Quantity Reward Magnitude Serial Learning 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Blumberg, M.S., & Wasserman, E.A. (1995). Animal mind and the argument from design. American Psychologist, 50, 133–144.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Burns, R. A., & Gordon, W. U. (1988). Some further observations on serial enumeration and categorical flexibility. Animal Learning & Behavior, 16, 425–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Burns, R. A., Hulbert, L. G., & Cribb, D. (1990). A test for order relevance in a three-element serial learning task. Journal of General Psychology, 117, 91–98.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Capaldi, E.J. (1986). Serial learning and trimethyltin: An unfortunate case of ad hoc conclusions. Physiological Psychology, 14, 71–72.Google Scholar
  5. Capaldi, E.J., & Miller, D.J. (1988). The rat’s simultaneous anticipation of remote events and current events can be sustained by event memories alone. Animal Learning & Behavior, 16, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Capaldi, E.J., & Molina, P. (1979). Element discriminability as a determinant of serial-pattern learning. Animal Learning & Behavior, 7, 318–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Capaldi, E.J, Nawrocki, T.M, Miller, D.J, & Verry, D.R. (1985). An examination into some variables said to affect serial learning. Animal Learning & Behavior, 13, 129–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Capaldi, E.J, Nawrocki, T.M, & Verry, D.R. (1982). Difficult serial anticipation learning in rats: Rule-encoding vs. memory. Animal Learning & Behavior, 10, 167–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Capaldi, E.J, Nawrocki, T.M, & Verry, D.R. (1983). The nature of anticipation: An inter- and intraevent process. Animal Learning & Behavior, 11, 193–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Capaldi, E.J, Verry, D.R, & Davidson, T. (1980a). Memory, serial anticipation pattern learning, and transfer in rats. Animal Learning & Behavior, 8, 575–585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Capaldi, E.J, Verry, D.R, & Davidson, T. (1980b). Why rule encoding by animals in serial learning remains to be established. Animal Learning & Behavior, 8, 691–692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Capaldi, E.J, Verry, D.R, Nawrocki, T.M, & Miller, D.J. (1984). Serial learning, interitem associations, phrasing cues, interference, overshadowing, chunking, memory, and extinction. Animal Learning & Behavior, 12, 7–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chen, S, Swartz, K.B, & Terrace, H.S. (1997). Knowledge of the ordinal position of list items in rhesus monkeys. Psychological Science, 8, 80–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Church, R.M. (1997). Quantitative models of animal learning and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 23, 379–389.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Church, R.M. (2001). A Turing test for computational and associative theories of learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 132–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Eich, J.M. (1982). A composite holographic associative recall model. Psychological Review, 89, 627–661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Eichenbaum, H., Otto, T., & Cohen, N.J. (1992). The hippocampus—What does it do? Behavioral and Neural Biology, 57, 2–36.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fountain, S.B. (1986). Serial-pattern learning: A unitary process? Physiological Psychology, 14, 67–70.Google Scholar
  19. Fountain, S.B. (1990). Rule abstraction, item memory, and chunking in rat serial-pattern tracking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 96–105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fountain, S.B., & Annau, Z. (1984). Chunking, sorting, and rule-learning from serial patterns of brain-stimulation reward by rats. Animal Learning & Behavior, 12, 265–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fountain, S.B., Evensen, J.C., & Hulse, S.H. (1983). Formal structure and pattern length in serial pattern learning by rats. Animal Learning & Behavior, 11, 186–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fountain, S.B., Henne, D.R., & Hulse, S.H. (1984). Phrasing cues and hierarchical organization in serial pattern learning by rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 30–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Fountain, S.B., & Hulse, S.H. (1981). Extrapolation of serial stimulus patterns by rats. Animal Learning & Behavior, 9, 381–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fountain, S.B., & Rowan, J.D. (1995a). Coding of hierarchical versus linear pattern structure in rats and humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21, 187–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Fountain, S.B., & Rowan, J.D. (1995b). Sensitivity to violations of “run” and “trill” structures in rat serial-pattern learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21, 78–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fountain, S.B., & Rowan, J.D. (2000). Differential impairments of rat serial-pattern learning and retention induced by MK-801, an NMDA receptor antagonist. Psychobiology, 28, 32–44.Google Scholar
  27. Fountain, S.B., Schenk, D.E., & Annau, Z. (1985). Serial-pattern-learning processes dissociated by trimethyltin exposure in rats. Physiological Psychology, 13, 53–62.Google Scholar
  28. Haggbloom, S.J. (1985). Serial learning and transfer in rats: effects of changes in stimulus-stimulus associations, pattern structure, and serial position information. Animal Learning & Behavior, 13, 370–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Haggbloom, S.J, & Brooks, D.M. (1985). Serial anticipation and pattern extrapolation in rats as a function of element discriminability. Animal Learning & Behavior, 13, 303–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hulse, S.H. (1978). Cognitive structure and serial pattern learning by animals. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior (pp. 311–340). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  31. Hulse, S.H. (1980). The case of the missing rule: Memory for reward vs. formal structure in serial-pattern learning by rats. Animal Learning & Behavior, 8, 689–690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hulse, S.H, & Campbell, C.E. (1975). “Thinking ahead” in rat discrimination learning. Animal Learning & Behavior, 3, 305–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hulse, S.H, & Dorsky, N.P. (1977). Structural complexity as a determinant of serial pattern learning. Learning and Motivation, 8, 488–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hulse, S.H, & Dorsky, N.P. (1979). Serial pattern learning by rats: Transfer of a formally defined stimulus relationship and the significance of nonreinforcement. Animal Learning & Behavior, 7, 211–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hunter, W.S. (1920). The temporal maze and kinaesthetic sensory processes in the white rat. Psychobiology, 2, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Jones, M.R. (1974). Cognitive representations of serial patterns. In B. Kantowitz (Ed.), Human information processing: Tutorials in performance and cognition, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  37. Lashley, K.S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112–146). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  38. Metcalfe, J. (1990). Composite holographic associative recall model (CHARM) and blended memories in eyewitness testimony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 145–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Metcalfe, J. (1993). Novelty monitoring, metacognition, and control in a composite holographic associative recall model: Implications for Korsakoff amnesia. Psychological Review, 100(1), 3–22.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Muller, M.D, Wallace, D.G, Smith, D.P, & Fountain, S.B. (2000). Predicting pattern violations using serial position in rat sequential learning. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 26, 2244.Google Scholar
  41. Murdock, B.B. (1982). A theory for the storage and retrieval of item and associative information. Psychological Review, 89, 609–626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Murdock, B.B. (1983). A distributed memory model for serial-order information. Psychological Review, 90, 316–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Murdock, B.B. (1985). Convolution and matrix systems: A reply to Pike. Psychological Review, 92, 130–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Olton, D.S., Shapiro, M.L, & Hulse, S.H. (1984). Working memory and serial patterns. In H. L. Roitblat, T. G. Bever, & H. S. Terrace (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 171–182). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  45. Restle, F. (1970). Theory of serial pattern learning: Structural trees. Psychological Review, 11, 481–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Restle, F. (1972). Serial patterns: The role of phrasing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 92, 385–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Restle, F. (1973). Serial pattern learning: Higher order transitions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 99, 61–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Restle, F., & Brown, E.R. (1970a). Organization of serial pattern learning. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  49. Restle, F., & Brown, E.R. (1970b). Serial pattern learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 120–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Restle, F., & Brown, E.R. (1970c). Serial pattern learning: Pretraining of runs and trills. Psychonomic Science, 19, 321–322.Google Scholar
  51. Restle, F., & Burnside, B.L. (1972). Tracking of serial patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 95, 299–307.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Roitblat, H.L. (1982). The meaning of representation in animal memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 353–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Roitblat, H.L., Bever, T.G., Heiweg, D.A., & Harley, H.E. (1991). On-line choice and the representation of serially structured stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 55–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Roitblat, H.L, Pologe, B, & Scopatz, R.A. (1983). The representation of items in serial position. Animal Learning & Behavior, 11, 489–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Roitblat, H.L, Scopatz, R, & Bever, T. (1987). The hierarchical representation of three-item sequences. Animal Learning & Behavior, 15, 179–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Skinner, B.F. (1934). The extinction of chained reflexes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 20, 234–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Stempowski, N.K, Carman, H.M, & Fountain, S.B. (1999). Temporal phrasing and overshadowing in rat serial-pattern learning. Learning and Motivation, 30, 74–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Terrace, H.S. (1987). Chunking by a pigeon in a serial learning task. Nature, 325, 149–151.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Terrace, H.S. (1991). Chunking during serial learning by a pigeon: I. Basic evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 81–93.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Terrace, H.S, & Chen, S. (1991a). Chunking during serial learning by a pigeon: II. Integrity of a chunk on a new list. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 94–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Terrace, H.S, & Chen, S. (1991b). Chunking during serial learning by a pigeon: III. What are the necessary conditions for establishing a chunk? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 107–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Wallace, D.G, & Fountain, S.B. (In press). What is learned in sequential learning? An associative model of reward magnitude serial-pattern learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes.Google Scholar
  63. Wathen, C.N, & Roberts, W.A. (1994). Multiple-pattern learning by rats on an eight-arm radial maze. Animal Learning & Behavior, 22, 155–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2002

Authors and Affiliations

  • Stephen B. Fountain
    • 1
  • Douglas G. Wallace
    • 1
  • James D. Rowan
    • 1
  1. 1.Kent State UniversityUSA

Personalised recommendations