Skip to main content

Word Learning and Word Acquisition

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Psychology ((BRIEFSCOGNIT))

Abstract

The chapter illustrates evidence on word learning and word acquisition which supports the WAT proposal. First, we outline approaches that emphasize the importance of social aspects; then, we turn to embodied approaches highlighting the role of perception and action in word acquisition. We then describe hybrid approaches, according to which the role of perception and action cues might have more weight in the early phases of word learning, while the role of social and linguistic cues might be prominent later, once children master some social abilities and possess a consistent vocabulary. We then describe studies on modality of acquisition, showing that concrete and abstract words are learned through different strategies, based on perception versus based on language. Finally, we report some acquisition studies with adults realized in our laboratory. Based on the reported theories and evidence, we argue that the acquisition of both concrete and abstract words rests on associative mechanisms. Perceptive salience, embodied attention, and bodily actions contribute to learning of concrete words associating words to their referents. However, for acquiring abstract words, a mechanism based on words to referents associations is more difficult to apply, given the sparse variety of referents abstract words have. This makes the social and the linguistic input very relevant for abstract word learning. Thus, abstract words are learned both associating words and referents and words to other words. These different associative mechanisms influence not only the conceptual representation but also the body, leading to the activation of different effectors, hand, and mouth.

I have always tried to write in a simple way, using down-to-earth and not abstract words.

Georges Simenon

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Baldwin, D. (1993). Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 2, 395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barca, L., Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2002). Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for Italian nouns. Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 34(3), 424–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergen, B. (2012). Louder than words: The new science of how the mind makes meaning. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergen, B., Narayan, S., & Feldman, J. (2003). Embodied verbal semantics: evidence from an image-verb matching task. Proceedings of the Twenty-Fifth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birch, S., Vauthier, S., & Bloom, P. (2008). Three- and four-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning. Cognition, 107, 1018–1034.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., Flumini, A., Cimatti, F., Marocco, D., & Scorolli, C. (2011). Manipulating objects and telling words: A study on concrete and abstract words acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 15. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00015

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Harvard: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corriveau, K., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Choosing your informant: Weighing familiarity and recent accuracy. Developmental Science, 12(3), 426–437.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A. (2009). Learning from others: Children’s construction of concepts. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 115–140.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. (2006). Why verbs are hard to learn. In K. Hirsch-Pasek & R. Golinkoff (Eds.), Action meets words: How children learn verbs (pp. 544–564). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Bíró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56(2), 165–193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L. R., & Lederer, A. (1999). Human simulations of vocabulary learning. Cognition, 73, 135–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman, L. R., Cassidy, K., Papafragou, A., Nappa, R., & Trueswell, J. T. (2005). Hard words. Journal of Language Learning and Development, 1(1), 23–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2006). Baby Wordsmith: From associationist to social sophisticate. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 30–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, R. L., & Gerken, L. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70, 109–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gómez, R. L., & Gerken, L. A. (2000). Infant artificial language learning and language acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4(5), 178–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Granito, C., Scorolli, C., & Borghi, A.M. (in preparation). Alice in Legoland: A study on abstract words acquisition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L., & Koenig, M. A. (2006). Trust in testimony: How children learn about science and religion. Child Development, 77, 505–524.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Hennon, E. A., & Maguire, M. J. (2004). Hybrid theories at the frontier of developmental psychology: The emergentist coalition model of word learning as a case in point. In D. Geoffrey Hall & S. R. Waxman (Eds.), Weaving a lexicon (pp. 173–204). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hollich, G. (2000). An emergentist coalition model for word learning: Mapping words to objects is a product of the interaction of multiple cues. In R. M. Golinkoff, K. Hirsh-Pasek, L. Bloom, L. B. Smith, A. L. Woodard, N. Akhtar, M. Tomasello, & G. Hollich (Eds.), Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 179–186). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollich, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R.M. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Society for Research in Child Development Monograph Series. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaakkola, R. O., & Slaughter, V. (2002). Children’s body knowledge: Understanding ‘life’ as a biological goal. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 325–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpov, Y. V., & Bransford, J. D. (1995). LS Vygotsky and the doctrine of empirical and theoretical learning. Educational Psychologist, 30(2), 61–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3, 299–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lany, J., & Saffran, J. R. (2010). From statistics to meaning: Infants, acquisition of lexical categories. Psychological Science, 21(2), 284–291. doi:10.1177/0956797609358570

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. (1989). Categorization and naming in young children. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D. L., & Atran, S. (2004). The native mind: Biological categorization and reasoning in development and across cultures. Psychological Review, 111, 960–983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Medina, T. N., Snedeker, J., Trueswell, J. C., & Gleitman, L. R. (2011). How words can and cannot be learned by observation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 9014–9019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1988). Constraints on word learning? Cognitive Development, 3, 221–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291–310. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291. PMID 11381831.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (2013). Dual coding theory, word abstractness, and emotion: A critical review of Kousta et al. (2011). Journal of Experimental Psychology, General, 142(1), 282–7. doi: 10.1037/a0027004.

  • Prinz, J. (2012). Beyond human nature: How culture and experience shape our lives. London, Penguin, New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pruden, S. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hennon, E. A. (2006). The birth of words: Ten-month-olds learn words through perceptual salience. Child Development, 77(2), 266–280.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Romberg, A. R., & Saffran, J. R. (2010). Statistical learning and language acquisition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Review: Cognitive Science, 1(6), 906–914.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development, 72, 1054–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. (1991). Thinking through cultures. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B. (1999). Children’s noun learning: How general learning processes make specialized learning mechanisms. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), The emergence of language (pp. 227–305). Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B. (2000). Learning how to learn words: An associative crane. In R. M. Golinkoff, K. Hirsh-Pasek, L. Bloom, L. Smith, A. Woodward, N. Akhtar, M. Tomasello, & G. Hollich (Eds.), Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B. (2005). Action alters shape categories. Cognitive Science, 29, 665–679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition, 106(3), 1558–1568.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snedeker, J., & Gleitman, L. (2004). Why it is hard to label our concepts? In D. G. Hall & S. R. Waxman (Eds.), Weaving a Lexicon (pp. 603–636). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tizard, B., & Hughes, M. (1984). Young children learning. London: Fontana.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1992). The social basis of language acquisition. Social Development, 1(1), 67–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2000). First steps toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cognitive Linguistics, 11(1–2), 61–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Akhtar, N. (1995). Two-year-olds use pragmatic cues to differentiate reference to objects and actions. Cognitive Development, 10, 201–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Akhtar, N. (2000). Five questions for any theory of word learning. In R. M. Golinkoff, K. Hirsh-Pasek, L. Bloom, L. B. Smith, A. L. Woodward, N. Akhtar, M. Tomasello, & G. Hollich (Eds.), Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 179–186). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–691.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S. (1994). Universal and culture-specific properties of children’s mental models of the earth. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 412–430). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (1981) The genesis of higher mental functions. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of activity in Soviet psychology. Armonk, NY: Sharpe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In R. W. Rieber & A. S. Carton (Eds.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, Volume 1: Problems of general psychology, (Trans. N. Minick). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wauters, L. N., Tellings, A. E. J. M., van Bon, W. H. J., & Mak, W. M. (2008). Mode of acquisition as a factor in deaf children’s reading comprehension. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13(2), 175–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wauters, L. N., Tellings, A. E. J. M., van Bon, W. H. J., & van Haaften, A. W. (2003). Mode of acquisition of word meanings: The viability of a theoretical construct. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 385–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weizman, Z. O., & Snow, C. E. (2001). Lexical output as related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology, 37(2), 265–279. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.2.265

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wojcik, E. H., & Saffran, J. R. (2013). The ontogeny of lexical networks: Toddlers encode the relationships among referents when learning novel words? Psychological Science, 24(10), 1898–1905.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, A. L., Markman, E., & Fitzsimmons, C. M. (1994). Rapid word learning in 13- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 30, 553–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2007). Rapid word learning under uncertainty via cross-situational statistics. Psychological Science, 18(5), 414–420.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. Cognition, 125, 244–262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anna M. Borghi .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Borghi, A.M., Binkofski, F. (2014). Word Learning and Word Acquisition. In: Words as Social Tools: An Embodied View on Abstract Concepts. SpringerBriefs in Psychology(). Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-9539-0_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics