Abstract

Working memory (WM) is usually defined as a cognitive system that provides temporary storage and manipulation of the information necessary for such complex cognitive tasks as language comprehension, learning, and reasoning; WM requires the simultaneous storage and processing of information (Baddeley 1992).

Keywords

Bilingual memory Digit span Second language learning Semantic span Working memory 

Notes

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to Natasha Aiguesvives for her support in editing this paper.

References

  1. Anderson, U. (2010). The contribution of working memory capacity to foreign language comprehension in children. Memory, 18, 458–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ardila, A. (2003). Language representation and working memory with bilinguals. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 233–240.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., & Puente, A. (1994). Neuropsychological evaluation of the Spanish speaker. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Ostrosky-Solis, F., Marcos, J., Granda, G., & Soto, M. (2000). Syntactic comprehension, verbal memory and calculation abilities in Spanish-English bilinguals. Applied Neuropsychology, 7, 3–16.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Ardila, A., & Rosselli, M. (2007). Neuropsicología clínica [clinical neuropsychology]. Mexico: El Manual Moderno.Google Scholar
  6. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556–559.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Baddeley A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Baddeley, A. D. (2001). Is working memory still working? American Psychologist, 56, 851–867.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  11. Baddeley, A. D., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. S., & Sivan, A. B. (1994). Multilingual aphasia examination (3rd ed.). Iowa City: AJA Associates Inc.Google Scholar
  13. Bledowski, C., Rahm, B., & Rowe, J. B. (2009). What ‘works’ in working memory? Separate systems for selection and updating of critical information. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 13735–13741.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Chee, M. W., Soon, C. S., Lee, H. L., & Pallier, C. (2004). Left insula activation: A marker for language attainment in bilinguals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 15265–15270.Google Scholar
  15. Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (1987). California verbal learning test. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  16. D’Esposito, M., & Postle, B. R. (2002). The neural basis of working memory storage, rehearsal, and control processes: Evidence from patient and functional magnetic imaging studies. In L. R. Squire & D. L. Schacter (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (3rd ed., pp. 215–224). New York: The Guildford Press.Google Scholar
  17. D’Esposito, M., Postle, B. R., Ballard, D., & Lease, J. (1999). Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: An event-related fMRI study. Brain and Cognition, 41, 66–86.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ellis, N. (1992). Linguistic relativity revisited: The bilingual word-length effect in working memory during counting, remembering numbers, and mental calculations. In R. J. Harris (Ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 137–155). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Ganschow, L., Sparks, R. L., Javrosky, J., Pohlman, J., & Bishop-Mabury, A. (1991). Identifying naïve language difficulties among foreign learners in college: A foreign language learning disability? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 530–541.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hernández, A. E. (2009). Language switching in the bilingual brain: what’s next? Brain and Language, 109, 133–140.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hernández, M., Costa, A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2012). Escaping capture: Bilingualism modulates distraction from working memory. Cognition. 122, 37–50.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hummel, K. M. (2002). Second language acquisition and working memory. In F. Fabbro (Ed.), Advances in the neurolinguistics of bilingualism (pp. 95–117). Italy: Forum.Google Scholar
  23. Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V. (2009a). The bilingual advantage in novel word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 705–710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V. (2009b). Bilingualism reduces native-language interference during novel-word learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 829–835.Google Scholar
  25. Kim, J. J., Kim, M. S., Lee, J. S., Lee, D. S., Lee, M. C., & Kwon, J. S. (2002). Dissociation of working memory processing associated with native and second languages: PET investigation. Neuroimage, 15, 879–891.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. MacWhinney, B. (2005). A unified model of language acquisition. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 49–67). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Majerus, S., Belayachi, S., De Smedt, B., Leclercq, A. L., Martinez, T., Schmidt, C., Weekes, B., & Maquet, P. (2008). Neural networks for short-term memory for order differentiate high and low proficiency bilinguals. Neuroimage, 42, 1698–1713.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Mikaye, A., & Friedman, N. P. (1998). Individual differences in second language proficiency: Working memory as language aptitude. In A. F. Healy & L. E. Bourne (Eds.), Foreign language learning: Psycholinguistic studies in training and retention (pp. 339–364). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  29. Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 14, 187–202.Google Scholar
  30. Nell, V. (2000). Cross-cultural neuropsychological assessment: Theory and practice. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  31. Olazaran, J., Jacobs, D. M., & Stern, Y. (1996). Comparative study of visual and verbal short-term memory in English and Spanish speakers: Testing a linguistic hypothesis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 105–110.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1995). Verbal short-term memory and vocabulary learning in polyglots. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 98–107.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Perani, D. (2005). The neural basis of language talent in bilinguals. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 211–213.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Rai, M. K., Loschky, L. C., Harris, R. J., Peck, N. R., & Cook, J. G. (2011). Effects of stress and working memory capacity on foreign language readers’ inferential processing during comprehension. Language Learning, 61, 187–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Rinne, J. O., Tommola, J., Laine, M., Krause, B. J., Schmidt, D., Kaasinen, V., Teras, M., Sipila, H., & Sunnari, M. (2000). The translating brain: cerebral activation patterns during simultaneous interpreting. Neuroscience Letter, 294, 85–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, and foreing language learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 21–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Skehan, P. (1989). Individual differences in second language learning. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
  38. Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283, 1657–1661.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Wechsler, D. (1944). The measurement of adult intelligence. Baltimore: The Williams and Wilkins Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Suggested Readings

  1. Ardila, A., & Ramos, E. (2007). Speech and language disorders in bilinguals. New York: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
  2. Baddeley, A. D. (2001). Is working memory still working? American Psychologist, 56, 851–867.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E., (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 14), 187–202.Google Scholar
  5. Paradis, J., Genesee, G., & Crago, M. B. (2011). Dual language development and disorders: A handbook on bilingualism and second language learning (2nd ed.). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
  6. Lépine, R., Bernardin, S.,& Barrouillet, P. (2005). Attention switching and working memory spans. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 329–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Alfredo Ardila
    • 1
  1. 1.Florida International UniversityMiamiUSA

Personalised recommendations