Advertisement

Organismic Computing

Chapter

Abstract

Herein we entertain the prospect that engineered approaches to human computation can foster more effective collaborations than are possible today. It is commonly known that adding more people to a group effort eventually produces diminishing returns. Need this be the case? Recent evidence suggests that group efficacy is related less to the individuals in a group and more to the quality of their interactions. Furthermore, each person added to a larger group creates many more possible pairwise relationships than adding a person to a smaller group does. Taken together, this would seem to suggest the opposite of what is observed, that there should be increasing returns when adding people to a group. That there are not implies that the costs associated with adding people to a group accrue faster than the benefits. These considerations compel an amelioration strategy that involves both increasing the value and decreasing the burden of group interactions. Toward that end, a new human computation paradigm is proposed, inspired by the successes of natural systems. This “organismic computing” approach seeks to improve collaboration efficacy via the affordances of shared sensing, collective reasoning, and coordinated action. In addition, a technique involving simulated augmented reality is introduced to enable a pilot study that compares organismic computing to other collaboration methods within a virtual environment. Results from this study point to increasing rather than decreasing returns for larger groups under this new collaboration model.

Keywords

Group Size Cognitive Load Augmented Reality Cognitive Architecture Jigsaw Puzzle 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

The author wishes to express deep gratitude to Kshanti Greene and Thomas Young of Social Logic Institute for their creative contributions and tireless execution of the present study as well as their helpful feedback on this chapter. The author would also like to acknowledge Geoffrey Bingham for his insightful comments regarding the application of ecological perception to distributed groups. Finally, the author would like to thank James Donlon for his enduring confidence and support of this speculative work. This research was funded under DARPA contract #D11AP00291.

References

  1. Anderson A (2011, November 17) Science: brain work. The economist. Retrieved from http://www.economist.com/node/21537050
  2. Bassett DS, Gazzaniga MS (2011) Understanding complexity in the human brain. Trends Cogn Sci 15(5):200–209. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bever T (1992) The demons and the beast: modular and nodular kinds of knowledge. In: Reilly R, Sharkey N (eds) Connectionist approaches to natural language processing. Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc, Hove, pp 213–252Google Scholar
  4. Blumberg M (2013) Patterns of connection. In: Michelucci P (ed) The handbook of human computation. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  5. Bruer JT (1999) Neural connections: some you use some you lose. Phi Delta Kappan 81(4):264–277Google Scholar
  6. Emes RD, Pocklington AJ, Anderson CNG, Bayes A, Collins MO, Vickers CA, Grant SGN (2008) Evolutionary expansion and anatomical specialization of synapse proteome complexity. Nat Neurosci 11(7):799–806. doi: 10.1038/nn.2135 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Gibson JJ (1986) The ecological approach to visual perception. Psychology PressGoogle Scholar
  8. Gibson EJ, Pick AD (2000) An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  9. Greene K, Young T (2013) Building blocks for collective problem solving. In: Michelucci P (ed) The handbook of human computation. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  10. Herculano-Houzel S (2009) The human brain in numbers: a linearly scaled-up primate brain. Front Hum Neurosci 3:31. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.031.2009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hernando A, Villuendas D, Vesperinas C, Abad M, Plastino A (2009) Unravelling the size distribution of social groups with information theory on complex networks (arXiv e-print No. 0905.3704). Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/0905.3704
  12. Heylighen F (2014) From human computation to the global brain: the self-organization of distributed intelligence. In: Michelucci PE (ed) Handbook of human computation. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  13. Hingston PF, Barone LC, Michalewicz Z (2008) Design by evolution: advances in evolutionary design. SpringerGoogle Scholar
  14. Koch C, Segev I (2000) The role of single neurons in information processing. Nat Neurosci 3:1171–1177. doi: 10.1038/81444 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kunegis J (2011, July 25) The density of a network is independent of its size. Netw Sci. Blog. Retrieved from http://networkscience.wordpress.com/2011/07/25/the-density-of-a-network-is-independent-of-its-size/, 2 July 2013
  16. Lasecki W, Bigham J (2014) Interactive crowds: real-time crowdsourcing and crowd agents. In: Michelucci P (ed) The handbook of human computation. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  17. Lin A, Huynh A, Barrington L, Lanckriet G (2013) Search and discovery through human computation. In: Michelucci P (ed) The handbook of human computation. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  18. Meier P (2013) Human computation for disaster response. In: Michelucci P (ed) The handbook of human computation. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  19. Moses A (2012) A mind’s eye in front of your nose. Syd Morning Her. Retrieved from http://www.smh.com.au/digital-life/digital-life-news/a-minds-eye-in-front-of-your-nose-20121123-29xhe.html, 7 July 2013
  20. Novak J (2013) Collective action and human computation: from crowd-workers to social collectives. In: Michelucci P (ed) The handbook of human computation. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  21. Oltramari A, Lebiere C (2012) Using ontologies in a cognitive-grounded system: automatic action recognition in video surveillance. In: Proceedings of the 7th international conference on semantic technology for intelligence, defense, and security, FairfaxGoogle Scholar
  22. Pavlic T, Pratt S (2014) Superorganismic behavior via human computation. In: Michelucci P (ed) The handbook of human computation. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  23. Sutcliffe A, Dunbar R, Binder J, Arrow H (2012) Relationships and the social brain: integrating psychological and evolutionary perspectives. Br J Psychol 103(2):149–168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Townsend JT (1990) Serial vs. parallel processing: sometimes they look like tweedledum and tweedledee but they can (and Should) be distinguished. Psychol Sci 1(1):46–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00067.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Van Essen DC, Ugurbil K, Auerbach E, Barch D, Behrens TE, Bucholz R, Chang A, Chen L, Corbetta M, Curtiss SW, Della Penna S, Feinberg D, Glasser MF, Harel N, Heath AC, Larson-Prior L, Marcus D, Michalareas G, Moeller S, Oostenveld R, Petersen SE, Prior F, Schlaggar BL, Smith SM, Snyder AZ, Xu J, Yacoub E, for the WU-Minn HCP Consortium (2012) The human connectome project: a data acquisition perspective. NeuroImage 62(4):2222–2231Google Scholar
  26. Van Raan AFJ (2013) Universities scale like cities. PLoS One 8(3):e59384. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059384 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Walker SI, Davies PCW (2012) The algorithmic origins of life (arXiv e-print No. 1207.4803). Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/1207.4803
  28. Woolley AW, Hashmi N (2014) Cultivating collective intelligence in online groups. In: Michelucci P (ed) The handbook of human computation. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  29. Woolley AW, Chabris CF, Pentland A, Hashmi N, Malone TW (2010) Evidence for a collective intelligence factor in the performance of human groups. Science 330(6004):686–688. doi: 10.1126/science.1193147 CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.FairfaxUSA

Personalised recommendations