Skip to main content

Déjà Vu: William James on “The Brain and the Mind,” 1878 – A Comment on Current Trends in Neurophenomenology Defining the Application of James’s Radical Empiricism to Psychology

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Neurophenomenology and Its Applications to Psychology

Abstract

William James was a man 150 years ahead of his time; meaning, he is still beyond us even today. At the very end of his life, he enjoined science to study the “fall of the threshold of consciousness, ‘tho’ we may not understand what we are looking at either in this generation or the next.” Contemporary neuroscience is only now beginning to awaken to James’s metaphysics, particularly his radical empiricism, as central to addressing the so-called Hard Problem—the relation between the brain and the mind. These range from the “biochemical theology” of Francis Crick to Francisco Varela’s neurophenomenology based on an embodied or enactive approach. Understood in the context of new developments in neurophenomenology, James’s radical empiricism then appears to have direct application to the future of scientific psychology.

It has seemed to me that the 6 hours that the trustees of the Lowell Fund have done me the honor to place at my disposal, could not be better spent than in taking a single subject, the brain, and seeing exactly how much recent investigations have explained its action and particular how much they may be said to have cleared up or made less mysterious the action of consciousness in each one of us. (William James, Manuscript Lectures, 1988, p. 31)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Baker, D. B. (Ed.). (2012). The Oxford handbook of the history of psychology: Global perspectives. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. A. (2007). Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(4), 485–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Paolo, E. A., Rohde, M., & Jaegher, H. (2007). Horizons for the enactive mind: Values, social interaction, and play. In J. Stewart, O. Gapenne, & E. A. Paolo (Eds.), Enaction: Towards a new paradigm for cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frankl, V. E. (1959). From death-camp to existentialism: A psychiatrist’s path to a new therapy. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (1998). The inordinance of time. Evanston, Ill: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Giorgi, A. (1970). Psychology as a human science: A phenomenologically based approach. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, S. (2009b). Radical empiricism and the neurophenomenology of time. Unpublished working paper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1900–1901). Logische Untersuchungen. Zweiter Teil: Untersuchungen zur Phänomenologie und Theorie der Erkenntnis. [Logical investigations. 2 vols.: Studies in phenomenology; The theory of knowledge.] Halle a.S.: Max Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1865). Review of Alfred R. Wallace’s The origin of human races and the antiquity of man deduced from the theory of ‘natural selection.’ North American Review, 100, 290–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1868). Review (unsigned) of Charles Darwin’s The variation of animals and plants under domestication. Atlantic Monthly, 22, 122–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1874). Notice (unsigned) of J. B. Petigrew’s Physiology of the circulation in plants, in lower animals, and in man. Nation, 19, 63.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1875). Notice (unsigned) of F. Papillon’s Nature and life. Nation, 20, 429.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1876). Note (unsigned) on the Revue Philosophique. Nation, 22, 147.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1877). Review (unsigned) on the Mind and the Brain (D Ferrier, H. Maudsley, J. Luys). Nation, 2J h, 355–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1879a). Are we automata? Mind, 4, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1879b). The sentiment of rationality. Mind, 4, 317–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (2 vols.). New York, NY: Henry Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1892). Psychology: Briefer course. New York, NY: Henry Holt.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1895). The tigers in India. Psychological Review, 2, 105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1897). The will to believe and other essays in popular philosophy. New York, NY: Longmans, Green & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1898, September). Philosophical conceptualizations and practical results. University of California Chronicle, 1(4), 24.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience: A study in human nature. New York, NY: Longmans, Green and Co.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1904a). A world of pure experience. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 1(21), 533543, 561–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1904b). Does “consciousness” exist? Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 1(18), 477491.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1907). Pragmatism. New York, NY: Longmans, Green, and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1988). The 1878 Lowell Lectures. In F. Burkhardt, F. Bowers, & I. K. Skrupskeis (Eds.), Manuscript lectures. The works of William James. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. (Ed). (1958–1964). Psychology: The study of a science (7 vols.). New York, NY: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., & Thompson, E. (2003). Neurophenomenology: Integrating subjective experience and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. Journal of consciousness Studies, 10(9–10), 31–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. R., & Varela, F. J. (1987). The tree of knowledge: The biological roots of human understanding. Boston, MA: New Science Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, R. (Ed.). (1961). Existential psychology. New York, NY: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, R., Angel, E., & Ellenberger, H. F. (Eds.). (1958). Existence: A new dimension in psychiatry and psychology. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott J. J. (Ed.). (1977). Writings of William James: A comprehensive edition. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moustakas, C. E. (1961). Loneliness. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidis, B. (1898). The psychology of suggestion. New York, NY: Appleton and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taves, A. (1999). Fits, trances, and visions: Experiencing religion and explaining experience from Wesley to James. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I. (1973). Psychological suspended animation: Heart rate, blood pressure, time estimation, and introspective reports from an anechoic environment. Unpublished MA Thesis, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, TX.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I. (1990). William James on Darwin: An evolutionary theory of consciousness. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 602, 7–33. Originally presented as part of the Darwin Centennial Lectures.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I. (1992b). The case for a uniquely American Jamesian tradition in psychology. In M. Donnelly (Ed.), Reinterpreting the legacy of William James (APA Centennial William James Lectures). (pp. 3–28).Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I. (2000). “What is man, Psychologist, that thou art so unmindful of him?”: Henry A, Murray on the historical relation between classical personality theory and humanistic psychology. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 40(3), 29–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I. (2003a). Why Wundt’s laboratory was not founded until 1950, Or the hypothesis of the three streams. The Wallace Russell Memorial Lecture presented to (Division 26) The History of Psychology at the 109th annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada, August 9, 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I. (2003b). Have we engaged in a colossal misreading of James’s Varieties? Streams of William James. Newsletter/journal of the William James Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I. (2010a). William James on exceptional mental states: Reconstruction of the 1896 unpublished Lowell lectures (2nd printing). Portsmouth, NH: Jetty Press. (Original work published 1982)

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I. (2010b). William James on an intuitive, phenomenologically oriented psychology in the immediate moment: The true foundation for a science of consciousness? Special issue on Consciousness in the Neurosciences. History of the Human Sciences, 23(3), 119–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I. (2010c). William James and the humanistic implications of the neuroscience revolution: An outrageous hypothesis. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 50(4), 410–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I. (2012a). The ontological triad in James and Peirce: A moment in The history of consciousness, Revue Internationale de Philosophie, 260, 177–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I. (2012b). The stream of consciousness: Literary psychology as the first uniquely American phenomenology in the works of William James and his Swedenborgian and Transcendentalist milieu. In A.T. Tymieniecka (Ed.), Art, Literature and Passions of the Skies, Analecta Husserliana, 108, 112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. I., Martinez, S., & Martin, F. (Eds.). (2000). Old Saybrook One: Benchmark in the history of humanistic psychology. A commemorative edition. Cambridge, MA: The Essene Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E. (2005). Sensorimotor subjectivity and the enactive approach to experience. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 407–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tymienieka, A.T. (1989). The theme: The history of American phenomenology-in-process. In E. F. Kaelin & C. O. Schrag (Eds.), American phenomenology: Origins and development, Analecta Hussereliana, 26, (ix-xxvii). Brattleboro, VT: World Institute for Advanced Phenomenological Research and Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J. (1996). Neurophenomenology. A methodological remedy for the hard problem. Journal of Consciousness studies, 3(4), 330–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J. (1999). The specious present. A neurophenomenology of time consciousness. In J. Petitot, F. J. Varela., B. Pachoud, & J-M. Roy (Eds.), Naturalizing phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science (pp. 266–314). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J., & Shear, J. (Eds.). (1999b). The view from within: First-person approaches to the study of consciousness. Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Weeks, E. (1966). The Lowells and their institute. Boston, MA: Little, Brown and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woods, J. H. (Trans.) (1917). The yoga sutras of Patanjali. Harvard Oriental Series, 17, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (2001). Beyond empathy: Phenomenological approaches to intersubjectivity, Journal of Consciousness studies, 8(5–7), 151–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (2005). Subjectivity and selfhood. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/The MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D., & Parnas, J. (1999). Phenomenal consciousness and self-awareness: A phenomenological critique of representational theory. In S. Gallagher & L. Shear (Eds.), Models of the self (pp. 253–270). Exeter, UK: Imprint Academic.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eugene Taylor Ph.D. .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Taylor, E. (2013). Déjà Vu: William James on “The Brain and the Mind,” 1878 – A Comment on Current Trends in Neurophenomenology Defining the Application of James’s Radical Empiricism to Psychology. In: Gordon, S. (eds) Neurophenomenology and Its Applications to Psychology. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-7239-1_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics