Skip to main content

Motivated Mind Perception: Treating Pets as People and People as Animals

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation ((NSM,volume 60))

Abstract

Human beings have a sophisticated ability to reason about the minds of others, often referred to as using one’s theory of mind or mentalizing. Just like any other cognitive ability, people engage in reasoning about other minds when it seems useful for achieving particular goals, but this ability remains disengaged otherwise. We suggest that understanding the factors that engage our ability to reason about the minds of others helps to explain anthropomorphism: cases in which people attribute minds to a wide range of nonhuman agents, including animals, mechanical and technological objects, and supernatural entities such as God. We suggest that engagement is guided by two basic motivations: (1) the motivation to explain and predict others’ actions, and (2) the motivation to connect socially with others. When present, these motivational forces can lead people to attribute minds to almost any agent. When absent, the likelihood of attributing a mind to others, even other human beings, decreases. We suggest that understanding the factors that engage our theory of mind can help to explain the inverse process of dehumanization, and also why people might be indifferent to other people even when connecting to them would improve their momentary wellbeing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abraham, A.T., Pocheptsova, A., Ferraro, R. (2012). The effect of mobile phone use on prosocial behavior. Unpublished manuscript, University of Maryland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. A., Riggs, K. J., Simpson, A., Chiavarino, C., & Samson, D. (2006). Is belief reasoning automatic? Psychological Science, 17, 841–844.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2004). Religion’s evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 713–770.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Avery, M. E. (1993). Preface. In K. J. S. Anand & P. J. McGrath (Eds.), Pain in neonates. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aydin, N., Fischer, P., & Frey, D. (2010). The effects of social exclusion on religiousness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 742–753.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aydin, N., Krüger, J., Fischer, J., Hahn, D., Frey, D., Kastenmüller, A., et al. (2012). A man´s best friend—how the presence of a dog decreases mental distress after social exclusion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 446–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press/Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J., & Keysar, B. (2004). Making sense of how we make sense: The paradox of egocentrism in language use. In H. L. Colston & A. N. Katz (Eds.), Figurative language comprehension: Social and cultural influences. Mahwaw: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2010). Excluded from humanity: the dehumanizing effects of social ostracism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 107–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Masicampo, E. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2011). Do conscious thoughts cause behavior? Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 331–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bering, J. M. (2006). The cognitive science of souls: Clarifications and extensions of the evolutionary model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 486–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 125, 1839–1849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chamberlain, B., Kasari, C., & Rotheram-Fuller, E. (2007). Involvement or isolation. The social networks of children with autism in regular classrooms. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 230–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012a). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 231–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chevallier, C., Molesworth, C., & Happé, F. (2012b). Diminished social motivation negatively impacts reputation management: autism spectrum disorders as a case in point. PLoS ONE, 7, e31107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague/Paris: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Citizens United v. Federal Election Commission, 558 U.S. 08–205 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W. A., & Brosch, T. (2012). Motivational salience: amygdala tuning from traits, needs, values, and goals. Psychological Science, 21(1), 54–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dando-Collins, S. (2004). Standing Bear is a person: the true story of a Native American’s quest for justice. Cambridge: Da Capo Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Demoulin, S., Leyens, J. P., Paladino, M. P., Rodriguez, R. T., Rodriguez, A. P., & Dovidio, J. F. (2004). Dimensions of “uniquely” and “nonuniquely” human emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 71–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13, 80–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N. & Schroeder, J. (2012). Mistakenly seeking solitude. Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N. (2008). Solving the (real) other minds problem. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1455–1474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N., & Waytz, A. (2010). Mind perception. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindsay (Eds.), The Handbook of Social Psychology (5th ed., pp. 498–541). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N., Akalis, S., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008a). Creating social connection through inferential reproduction: Loneliness and perceived agency in gadgets, gods, and greyhounds. Psychological Science, 19, 114–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N., Waytz, A., Akalis, S., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008b). When we need a human: Motivational determinants of anthropomorphism. Social Cognition, 26, 143–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114, 864–886.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. J., & Heberlein, A. S. (2007). Personhood and neuroscience: Naturalizing or nihilating? American Journal of Bioethics, 7, 37–48.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feith, D. (2011). Will the lights go out in the NFL? The Wall Street Journal. Accessed at http://online.wsj.com/article/SB10001424052748704415104576066153465352540.html.

  • Fiske, S. T. (1993). Controlling other people: The impact of power on stereotyping. American Psychologist, 48, 621–628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fleeson, W., Malanos, A. B., & Achille, N. M. (2002). An intraindividual process approach to the relationship between extraversion and positive affect: Is acting extraverted as “good” as being extraverted? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1409–1422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gaunt, R., Leyens, J. P., & Demoulin, S. (2002). Intergroup relations and the attribution of emotions: control over memory for secondary emotions associated with ingroup versus outgroup. Journal of Experiment Social Psychology, 38, 508–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gebauer, J. E., & Maio, G. R. (2012). The need to belong can motivate belief in god. Journal of Personality, 80, 465–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T., Kruger, J., & Savitsky, K. (1999). Everyday egocentrism and everyday interpersonal problems. In R. M. Kowalski & M. R. Leary (Eds.), The social psychology of emotional and behavioral problems: Interfaces of social and clinical psychology (pp. 69–95). Washington, DC: APA Books.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Goff, P. A., Eberhardt, J. L., Williams, M., & Jackson, M. C. (2008). Not yet human: Implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 292–306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, T., & Ickes, W. (1997). When women’s intuition isn’t greater than men’s. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 117–143). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, H. M., Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Dimensions of mind perception. Science, 315, 619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2010). Blaming God for our pain: human suffering and the divine mind. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 7–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruenfeld, D. H., Inesi, M. E., Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Power and the objectification of social targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 111–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., & Schmid-Mast, M. (2008). Are women always more interpersonally sensitive than men? Impact of goals and content domain. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 144–155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haque, O. S., & Waytz, A. (2012). Dehumanization in medicine: Causes, solutions, and functions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 176–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, L. T., & Fiske, L. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme outgroups. Psychological Science, 17, 847–853.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, M. A., & Hall, A. E. (2010). Anthropomorphism, empathy, and perceived communicative ability vary with phylogenetic relatedness to humans. The Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 4, 34–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, N., Loughnan, S., Holland, E. (2013). The psychology of humanness. In S.J. Gervais (Ed.), Objectification and dehumanization (pp. 25–52). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1946). Emotion in man and animal: an analysis of the intuitive processes of recognition. Psychological Review, 53, 88–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heberlein, A. S., & Adolphs, R. (2004). Perception in the absence of social attribution: selective impairment in anthropomorphizing following bilateral amygdala damage. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 7487–7491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 1360–1366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, J. B. (2000). The legacy of Luna: the story of a tree, a woman, and the struggle to save the Redwoods. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, A. (2009). Inside of a dog: What dogs see, smell, and know. New York: Scribner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 212–229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1957). The natural history of religion. Stanford: Stanford University Press (Original work published 1757).

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, N. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P. P. G. Bateson & R. A. Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology. Oxford: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, N. (2002). Bugs and beasts before the law. In N. Humphrey (Ed.), The Mind Made Flesh: Essays from the Frontiers of Psychology and Evolution (pp. 235–254). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W. (2003). Everyday mind reading: Understanding what other people think and feel. Amherst: Prometheus Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., Gesn, P. R., & Graham, T. (2000). Gender differences in empathic accuracy: Differential ability or differential motivation? Personal Relationships, 7, 95–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izuma, K., Matsumoto, K., Camerer, C. F., & Adolphs, R. (2011). Insensitivity to social reputation in autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 17302–17307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. C. (2003). Detecting agents. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 358, 549–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 946–954.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192–240). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kozak, M. J., Marsh, A. A., & Wegner, D. M. (2006). What do I think you’re doing? Action identification and mind attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 543–555.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kraus, M. W., Cote, S., & Keltner, D. (2010). Social class, contextualism, and empathic accuracy. Psychological Science, 21, 1716–1723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krienen, F. M., Tu, P., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Clan mentality: evidence that the medial prefrontal cortex responds to close others. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 13906–13915.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesher, J.H. (Trans.) (1992). Xenophanes of Colophon: fragments. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leyens, J. P., Paladino, P. M., Rodriguez, R. T., Vaes, J., Demoulin, S., Rodriguez, A. P., et al. (2000). The emotional side of prejudice: The role of secondary emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 186–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J. (1997). An essay concerning human understanding. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books (Original work published 1841).

    Google Scholar 

  • Luczak, H., Roetting, M., & Schmidt, L. (2003). Let’s talk: anthropomorphization as a means to cope with stress of interacting with technical devices. Ergonomics, 46, 1361–1374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., & Weisberg, J. (2003). Neural foundations for understanding social and mechanical concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 575–587.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McConnell, A. R., Brown, C. M., Shoda, T. M., Stayton, L. E., & Martin, C. E. (2011). Friends with benefits: On the positive consequences of pet ownership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 1239–1252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D. L., & Atran, S. (2004). The native mind: biological categorization and reasoning in development and across cultures. Psychological Review, 111, 960–983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morewedge, C.K. (2006). A mind of its own: Negativity bias in the perception of intentional agency. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morewedge, C. K., Preston, J., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Timescale bias in the attribution of mind. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R. S. (1999). How we know—and sometimes misjudge—what others know: imputing one’s own knowledge to others. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 737–759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science, 322, 58–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A. (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 239–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1929). The child’s conception of the world. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riby, D. M., & Hancock, P. J. B. (2008). Viewing it differently: social scene perception in Williams syndrome and autism. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2855–28600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Serpell, J. A. (2003). Anthropomorphism and anthropomorphic selection: beyond the “cute response”. Society & Animals, 11, 83–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, G. (1901). Three plays for puritans. Chicago and New York: Herbert S. Stone and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, G.D. & Chandler, J. (2012). Cuteness cues elicit anthropomorphism. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, G. D., & Haidt, J. (2011). Cuteness and disgust: the humanizing and dehumanizing effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 3, 245–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherrod, L. R. (1979). Social cognition in infants: attention to the human face. Infant Behavior and Development, 2, 279–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, P. W. (2009). Wired for war: The robotics revolution and conflict in the 21st century. New York: Penguin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitzer, G. (2011). Clever apes (Episode #19). Chicago: WBEZ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sproull, L., Subramani, M., Kiesler, S., Walker, J. H., & Waters, K. (1996). When the interface is a face. Human–Computer Interaction, 11, 97–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Universal Declaration of Human Rights (1948). United Nations Office of the High Commissioner for Human rights. Retrieved from http://www.un.org/en/documents/udhr/.

  • Van Kleef, G. A., Oveis, C., Van der Löwe, I., LuoKogan, A., Goetz, J., & Keltner, D. (2009). Power, distress, and compassion: turning a blind eye to the suffering of others. Psychological Science, 19, 1315–1322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wantanabe, S. (2007). How animal psychology contributes to animal welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 106, 193–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waytz, A., & Epley, N. (2012). Social connection enables dehumanization. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 70–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waytz, A., Cacioppo, J., Epley, N. (2012). Loneliness and anthropomorphic representations of the universe are positively correlated. Unpublished data, University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waytz, A., Cacioppo, J. T., & Epley, N. (2010a). Who sees human? The stability and importance of individual differences in anthropomorphism. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 219–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waytz, A., Epley, N., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010b). Social cognition unbound: psychological insights into anthropomorphism and dehumanization. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 58–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waytz, A., Gray, K., Epley, N., & Wegner, D. M. (2010c). Causes and consequences of mind perception. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14, 383–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waytz, A., Morewedge, C. K., Epley, N., Monteleone, G., Gao, J.-H., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010d). Making sense by making sentient: effectance motivation increases anthropomorphism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 410–435.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, X., Zuo, X., Wang, X., & Han, S. (2009). Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 8525–8529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicholas Epley .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Epley, N., Schroeder, J., Waytz, A. (2013). Motivated Mind Perception: Treating Pets as People and People as Animals. In: Gervais, S. (eds) Objectification and (De)Humanization. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, vol 60. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6959-9_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics