Skip to main content

Measures of Visuospatial Ability

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Assessing Literacy in Deaf Individuals

Abstract

This chapter presents the data related to a visuospatial functioning for the VL2 Psychometric Toolkit Project. The data were generated from a measure of visuospatial memory, the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R), and a mental rotation task, for a sample of deaf college students evaluated as part of the VL2 Psychometric Toolkit. In addition to providing descriptive statistics, correlations with a brief measure of nonverbal intelligence and measures of executive, memory, and linguistic functioning, and academic achievement administered concurrently as part of the VL2 Toolkit are presented. The relationships observed are discussed in the context of both research in the general population and, when available, previous research with deaf individuals.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Allen, B. J., & Gfeller, J. D. (2011). The immediate post-concussion assessment and cognitive testing battery and traditional neuropsychological measures: A construct and concurrent validity study. Brain Injury, 25(2), 179–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ariniello, L. (2000). Gender and the brain. Retrieved March 10, 2006, from web.sfn.org: web.sfn.org/content/Publications/BrainBrieffings/brain_lang_reading.htm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bavelier, D., Brozinsky, C., Tomann, A., Mitchell, T., & Liu, G. (2001). Impact of early deafness and early exposure to sign language on the cerebral organization for motion processing. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(22), 8931–8942.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelier, D., Dye, M. W. G., & Hauser, P. C. (2006). Do deaf individuals see better? Trends in Cognitive Science, 10, 512–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beglinger, L. J., Duff, K., Moser, D. J., Cross, S. A., & Kareken, D. A. (2009). The Indiana Faces in Places Test: Preliminary findings on a new visuospatial memory test in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 607–618.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benedict, R. H. B. (1997). Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R). Odessa, FL: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benedict, R. H. B., Schretlen, D., Groninger, L., Dobraski, M., & Shpritz, B. (1996). Revision of the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test: Studies of normal performance, reliability, and validity. Psychological Assessment, 8, 145–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosworth, R. G., & Dobkins, K. R. (1999). Left hemisphere dominance for motion processing in deaf signers. Psychological Science, 10(3), 256–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosworth, R. G., & Dobkins, K. R. (2002a). Visual field asymmetries for motion processing in deaf and hearing signers. Brain and Cognition, 49(1), 152–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bosworth, R. G., & Dobkins, K. R. (2002b). The effects of spatial selective attention on motion processing in deaf and hearing subjects. Brain and Cognition, 49(1), 170–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bosworth, R. G., Wright, C. E., Bartlett, M. S., Corina, D. P., & Dobkins, K. R. (2003). Characterization of the visual properties of signs in ASL. In A. E. Baker, B. van den Bogaerde, & O. Crasborn (Eds.), Cross-linguistic perspectives in sign language research. Hamburg: Signum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R., Pezaris, E., & Benbow, C. P. (1995). The influence of spatial ability on gender differences in mathematical college entrance test scores across diverse samples. Developmental Psychology, 31(4), 697–705.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chamberlain, C., & Mayberry, R. I. (2008). ASL syntactic and narrative comprehension in skilled and less skilled adult readers: Bilingual-bimodal evidence for the linguistic basis of reading. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 29, 368–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. A., Poppas, A., Forman, D. E., Hoth, K. F., Haley, A. P., Gunstad, J., et al. (2009). Vascular and cognitive functions associated with cardiovascular disease in the elderly. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(1), 96–110. doi:10.1080/13803390802014594.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeLana, M., Gentry, M., & Andrews, J. (2007). The efficacy of ASL/English bilingual education: Investigating public schools. American Annals of the Deaf, 152(1), 73–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dror, I., & Kosslyn, S. (1994). Mental imagery and aging. Psychology and Aging, 9(1), 90–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, K. (2002). The impact of sign language use on visuospatial cognition. In K. Emmorey (Ed.), Language, cognition and the brain: Insights from sign language research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, K., Klima, E., & Hickok, G. (1998). Mental rotation within linguistic and non-linguistic domains in users of American Sign Language. Cognition, 68(3), 221–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18(10), 850–855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freel, B., Clark, D., Anderson, M., Gilbert, G., Musyoka, M., & Hauser, P. (2011). Deaf individuals’ bilingual abilities: American sign language proficiency, reading skills, and family characteristics. Psychology, 2(1), 18–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halper, E. B. (2009). The nature of relationships between mental rotation, math, and English in deaf signers. Doctoral dissertation. Available from Dissertations and Theses database. (UMI Number: 3388611).

    Google Scholar 

  • Halper, E., Hall, W. C., & Morere, D. A. (2011, June). Short form of the mental rotation test with deaf participants. Poster presented at the American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology (AACN) 9th annual conference, Washington, DC. Abstract in The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25(4), 516–607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, P. C., Cohen, J., Dye, M. W. G., & Bavelier, D. (2007). Visual constructive and visual-motor skills in deaf native signers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12, 148–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., & Park, Y. (1990). Impact of practice on speed of mental rotation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49(2), 227–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., Pellegrino, J., & Carter, P. (1980). Developmental changes in mental rotation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 102–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, L. (2003a). Considerations for designing practice for deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8(2), 171–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, L. (2003b). The importance of processing automaticity and temporary storage capacity to the differences in comprehension between skilled and less skilled college-age deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8(3), 230–249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, R. (2008). Deaf learners and mathematical problem solving. In Deaf Cognition ­(pp. 226–249). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lim, W., Bardwell, W. A., Loredo, J. S., Kim, E., Ancoli-Israel, S., Morgan, E. E., et al. (2007). Neuropsychological effects of 2-week continuous positive airway pressure treatment and supplemental oxygen in patients with obstructive sleep apnea: A randomized placebo-controlled study. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 3(4), 380–386.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linacre, J. M. (1994). Sample size and item calibration stability. Rasch Measurement Transactions, 7(4), 328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2006). Study of mathematically precocious youth after 35 years: Uncovering antecedents for the development of math-science expertise. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 316–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marschark, M. (2003). Cognitive functioning in deaf adults and children. In M. Marschark & P. Spencer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (pp. 464–477). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A. J. (2010, September). Age of acquisition effects of American Sign Language on mental rotation. Poster presented at the Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research 2010 conference, West Lafayette, IN. Poster retrieved from http://www.purdue.edu/tislr10/pdfs/Martin%20AJ.pdf.

  • Masters, M., & Sanders, B. (1993). Is the gender difference in mental rotation disappearing? Behavior Genetics, 23(4), 337–341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moè, A., & Pazzaglia, F. (2010). Beyond genetics in mental rotation test performance: The power of effort attribution. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 464–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moores, D., & Sweet, C. (1990). Relationships of English grammar and communicative fluency to reading in deaf adolescents. Exceptionality, 1(2), 97–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Musselman, C. (2000). How do children who can’t hear learn to read an alphabetic script? A review of the literature on reading and deafness. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(1), 9–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neubauer, A., Bergner, S., & Schatz, M. (2010). Two- vs. three-dimensional presentation of mental rotation tasks: Sex differences and effects of training on performance and brain activation. Intelligence, 38(5), 529–539.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., & Moreno, C. (2002). An intervention program for promoting deaf pupils’ achievement in mathematics. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 7(2), 120–133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parasnis, I., Samar, V., Bettger, J., & Sathe, K. (1996). Does deafness lead to enhancement of visual spatial cognition in children? Negative evidence from deaf nonsigners. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1(2), 145–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C., & Sandak, R. (2000). Reading optimally builds on spoken language: Implications for deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(1), 32–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, M., Laeng, B., Latham, K., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, R., & Richardson, C. (1995). A redrawn Vandenburg & Kuse mental rotations test: Different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28, 39–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, L., Wynn, V., Gilhooly, K., Salla, S., & Logie, R. (1999). The role of memory in the tower of london task. Memory, 7(2), 209–231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prescott, J., Gavrilescu, M., Cunnington, R., O’Boyle, M. W., & Egan, G. F. (2010). Enhanced brain connectivity in math-gifted adolescents: An fMRI study using mental rotation. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(4), 277–288. doi:10.1080/17588928.2010.506951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Qi, S., & Mitchell, R. E. (2012). Large-scale academic achievement testing of deaf and hard-of-hearing students: Past, present, and future. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 17(1), 1–18. doi:10.1093/deafed/enr028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, D., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. (2001). Importance of assessing spatial ability in intellectually talented young adolescents: A 20-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 604–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strong, M., & Prinz, P. M. (1997). A study of the relationship between American Sign Language and English literacy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2(1), 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenburg, S., & Kuse, A. (1978). Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47(2), 599–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilbur, R. P. (2000). The use of ASL to support the development of English and literacy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(1), 81–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, B. D., & Stone, M. H. (1979). Best test design. Chicago: MESA Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Donna A. Morere .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Morere, D.A., Hall, W.C., Allen, T. (2012). Measures of Visuospatial Ability. In: Morere, D., Allen, T. (eds) Assessing Literacy in Deaf Individuals. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-5269-0_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics