Skip to main content

Reducing the Effect of Framed Messages About Health

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Transparent Communication of Health Risks

Abstract

Patients must be informed about risks before any treatment can be implemented. Yet serious problems in communicating these risks occur because of framing effects. In this chapter, we describe two studies conducted in the USA, Germany, and Spain, investigating the effects of different information frames when communicating health risks. Study 1 focused on people with low and high numeracy and investigated framing effects in perceptions of medical risks expressed in positive (i.e., chances of surviving after surgery) and negative (i.e., chances of dying after surgery) terms. Study 2 focused on a large sample of sexually active young adults and investigated framing effects in affective reactions, risk perceptions, attitudes, behavioral intentions, and reported behaviors relating to the prevention and detection of sexually transmitted diseases. Results in both studies showed that people are susceptible to framing effects and illustrate that these effects can be countered or eliminated by using different types of visual displays.

In this chapter, we partially reproduced the articles Garcia-Retamero, R., & Galesic, M. (2010). How to reduce the effect of framing on messages about health. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 25, 1323−1329, and Garcia-Retamero, R., & Cokely, E. T. (2011). Effective communication of risks to young adults: Using message framing and visual aids to increase condom use and STD screening. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17, 270−287.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    We focused on the consequences of conducting screening at different stages of a STD as this factor substantially influences both the impact of the symptoms and treatment effectiveness (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 2003; Wortley et al. 1995).

  2. 2.

     The Sobel test (see Sobel 1982) indicates whether the mediator significantly carries the influence of an independent variable to a dependent variable. That is, whether the indirect effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable through the mediator variable is significant.

References

  • Abood, D. A., Black, D. R., & Coster, D. C. (2005). Loss-framed minimal intervention increases mammography use. Women’s Health Issues, 15, 258–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abood, D. A., Coster, D. C., Mullis, A. K., & Black, D. R. (2002). Evaluation of a “loss-framed” minimal intervention to increase mammography utilization among medically un- and under-insured women. Cancer Detection and Prevention, 26, 394–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I., & Gilbert Cote, N. (2008). Attitudes and the prediction of behavior. In W. D. Crano & R. Prislin (Eds.), Attitudes and attitude change (pp. 289–311). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Almashat, S., Ayotte, B., Edelstein, B., & Margrett, J. (2008). Framing effect debiasing in medical decision making. Patient Education and Counseling, 71, 102–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Social Health Association (ASHA). (2005). State of the nation 2005: Challenges facing STD prevention in youth. Research Triangle Park, NC: ASHA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ancker, J. S., Senathirajah, Y., Kukafka, R., & Starren, J. B. (2006). Design features of graphs in health risk communication: A systematic review. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, 13, 608–618.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Apanovitch, A. M., McCarthy, D., & Salovey, P. (2003). Using message framing to motivate HIV testing among low-income, ethnic minority women. Health Psychology, 22, 60–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, K., Schwartz, J. S., Fitzgerald, G., Putt, M., & Ubel, P. A. (2002). Effect of framing as gain versus loss on understanding and hypothetical treatment choices: Survival and mortality curves. Medical Decision Making, 22, 76–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical consideration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bermudez, M. P., & Teva-Álvarez, I. (2003). Situación actual del VIH/SIDA en Europa: Análisis de las diferencias entre países. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 3, 89–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, L. G., & Keller, P. A. (1995). When to accentuate the negative: The effects of perceived efficacy and message framing on intentions to perform a health-related behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 32, 192–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruine de Bruin, W., & Fischhoff, B. (2000). The effect of question format on measured HIV/AIDS knowledge: Detention center teens, high school students, and adults. AIDS Education and Prevention, 12, 187–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2003). Late versus early testing of HIV—16 sites, United States, 2000–2003. MMWR Weekly Report, 52, 581–586. http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5225a2.htm, accessed July 30, 2012.

  • Chesson, H. W., Blandford, J. M., Gift, T. L., Tao, G., & Irwin, K. L. (2004). The estimated direct medical cost of sexually transmitted diseases among American youth, 2000. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 36, 11–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Child Trends (2006). Facts at a glance. Publication # 2006-03. http://www.childtrends.org/files/faag2006.pdf, accessed July 30, 2012.

  • Cleary, P. D., & Angel, R. (1984). The analysis of relationships involving dichotomous dependentvariables. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 25, 334–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cokely, E. T., & Kelley, C. M. (2009). Cognitive abilities and superior decision making under risk: A protocol analysis and process model evaluation. Judgment and Decision Making, 4, 20–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cokely, E. T., Kelley, C. M., & Gilchrist, A. H. (2006). Sources of individual differences in working memory: Contributions of strategy to capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 991–997.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • http://www.isciii.es/htdocs/pdf/grupotrabajoits.pdf

  • Corbin, J., McElroy, T., & Black, C. (2010). Memory reflected in our decisions: Higher working memory capacity predicts greater bias in risky choice. Judgment and Decision Making, 5, 110–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davids, S. L., Schapira, M. M., McAuliffe, T. L., & Nattinger, A. B. (2004). Predictors of pessimistic breast cancer risk perceptions in a primary care population. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 19, 310–315.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehne, K. L., & Riedner, G. (2005). Sexually transmitted infections among adolescents: The need for adequate health services. Geneva: World Health Organization and Deutsche Gesellschaft fuer Technische Zusammenarbeit. http://whqlibdoc.who.int/publications/2005/9241562889.pdf, accessed July 30, 2012.

  • Des Jarlais, D. C., Sloboda, Z., Friedman, S. R., Tempalski, B., McKnight, C., & Braine, N. (2006). Diffusion of the D.A.R.E. and syringe exchange programs. American Journal of Public Health, 96, 1354–1358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Detweiler, J. B., Bedell, B. T., Salovey, P., Pronin, E., & Rothman, A. J. (1999). Message framing and sunscreen use: Gain-framed messages motivate beach-goers. Health Psychology, 18, 189–196.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Downs, J. S., Bruine de Bruin, W., Murray, P. J., & Fischhoff, B. (2006). Specific STI knowledge may be acquired too late. Journal of Adolescent Health, 38, 65–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, A., Elwyn, G., Covey, J., Matthews, E., & Pill, R. (2001). Presenting risk information: A review of the effects of “framing” and other manipulations on patient outcomes. Journal of Health Communication, 6, 61–82.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, A., Elwyn, G., & Mulley, A. (2002). Explaining risks: Turning numerical data into meaningful pictures. British Medical Journal, 324, 827–830.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, A. G. K., Pill, R. M., & Stott, N. C. H. (1996). Communicating risk: Use of standard terms is unlikely to result in standard communication. British Medical Journal, 313, 1483.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • European Commission (2003). Health statistics: Key data on health 2002. Luxembourg: Office for Official Publications of the European Communities. http://epp.eurostat.ec.europa.eu/cache/ITY_OFFPUB/KS-08-02-002/EN/KS-08-02-002-EN.PDF, accessed July 30, 2012.

  • Fagerlin, A., Ubel, P. A., Smith, D. M., & Zikmund-Fisher, B. J. (2007). Making numbers matter: Present and future research in risk communication. American Journal of Health Behavior, 31, 47–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagerlin, A., Wang, C., & Ubel, P. A. (2005). Reducing the influence of anecdotal reasoning on people’s health care decisions: Is a picture worth a thousand statistics? Medical Decision Making, 25, 398–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galesic, M., Garcia-Retamero, R., & Gigerenzer, G. (2009). Using icon arrays to communicate medical risks: Overcoming low numeracy. Health Psychology, 28, 210–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Retamero, R., & Cokely, E. T. (2011). Effective communication of risks to young adults: Using message framing and visual aids to increase condom use and STD screening. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 17, 270–287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Retamero, R., Galesic, M., & Gigerenzer, G. (2010). Do icon arrays help reduce denominator neglect? Medical Decision Making, 30, 672–684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Retamero, R., Galesic, M., & Gigerenzer, G. (2011). Enhancing understanding and recall of quantitative information about medical risks: A cross-cultural comparison between Germany and Spain. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14, 213–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerend, M. A., & Cullen, M. (2008). Effects of message framing and temporal context on college student drinking behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1167–1173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerend, M. A., & Shepherd, J. E. (2007). Using message framing to promote acceptance of the human papillomavirus vaccine. Health Psychology, 26, 745–752.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Edwards, A. (2003). Simple tools for understanding risks: From innumeracy to insight. British Medical Journal, 327, 741–744.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., Gaissmaier, W., Kurz-Milcke, E., Schwartz, L. M., & Woloshin, S. (2007). Helping doctors and patients make sense of health statistics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8, 53–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haward, M. F., Murphy, R. O., & Lorenz, J. M. (2008). Message framing and perinatal decisions. Pediatrics, 122, 109–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Instituto Nacional de Estadística (INE) (2003). Encuesta de salud y hábitos sexuales, 2003: Resultados. http://www.ine.es/jaxi/menu.do?type=pcaxis&path=/t15/p455&file=inebase&L=0, accessed July 30, 2012

  • http://www.ine.es/jaxi/menu.do?type=pcaxis%26path=/t15/p455%26%ef%ac%81le=inebase%26L=0.

  • Johnson, E. J., Haubl, G., & Keinan, A. (2007). Aspects of endowment: A query theory of value construction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 461–474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision making under risk. Econometrica, 6, 621–630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The psychology of preferences. Scientific American, 46, 160–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S., Goldstein, D., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (2005). Framing effects in younger and older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60B, P215–P218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, D. B. (2008). The impact of abstinence and comprehensive sex and STD/HIV education programs on adolescent sexual behavior. Sexuality Research and Social Policy: A Journal of the NSRC, 5, 18–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, D., & Laris, B. A. (2009). Effective curriculum-based sex and STD/HIV education programs for adolescents. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 21–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, P. K., Manhart, L. E., & Lafferty, W. E. (2008). Abstinence-only and comprehensive sex education and the initiation of sexual activity and teen pregnancy. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42, 344–351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhberger, A. (1998). The influence of framing on risky decisions: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 75, 23–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Latimer, A. E., Rivers, S. E., Rench, T. A., Katulak, N. A., Hicks, A., Hodorowski, J. K., et al. (2008). A field experiment testing the utility of regulatory fit messages for promoting physical activity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 826–832.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lerman, C., Ross, E., Boyce, A., Gorchov, P. M., McLaughlin, R., Rimer, B., et al. (1992). The impact of mailing psychoeducational materials to women with abnormal mammograms. American Journal of Public Health, 82, 729–730.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76, 149–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I. P., Schnittjer, S. K., & Thee, S. L. (1988). Information framing effects in social and personal decisions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 520–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipkus, I. M. (2007). Numeric, verbal, and visual formats of conveying health risks: Suggested best practices and future recommendations. Medical Decision Making, 27, 696–713.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lipkus, I. M., & Hollands, J. G. (1999). The visual communication of risk. Journal of the National Cancer Institute. Monographs, 25, 149–163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lipkus, I. M., Samsa, G., & Rimer, B. K. (2001). General performance on a numeracy scale among highly educated samples. Medical Decision Making, 21, 37–44.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Llewellyn-Thomas, H. A., McGreal, M. J., & Thiel, E. C. (1995). Cancer patients’ decision making and trial-entry preferences: The effects of “framing” information about short-term toxicity and long-term survival. Medical Decision Making, 15, 4–12.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lunney, G. H. (1970). Using analysis of variance with a dichotomous dependent variable—Empirical study. Journal of Educational Measurement, 7, 263–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maheswaran, D., & Meyers-Levy, J. (1990). The influence of message framing and issue involvement. Journal of Marketing Research, 27, 361–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, T. L., Sherman, D. K., & Updegraff, J. A. (2004). Dispositional motivations and message framing: A test of the congruency hypothesis in college students. Health Psychology, 23, 330–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marteau, T. M. (1989). Framing of information: Its influence upon decisions of doctors and patients. British Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 89–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McNeil, B. J., Pauker, S. G., Sox, H. C., & Tversky, A. (1982). On the elicitation of preferences for alternative therapies. The New England Journal of Medicine, 306, 1259–1262.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meyerowitz, B. E., & Chaiken, S. (1987). The effect of message framing on breast self-examination attitudes, intentions, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 500–510.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Millar, M. G., & Millar, K. U. (2000). Promoting safe driving behaviors: The influence of message framing and issue involvement. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 853–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. M., & Fagley, N. S. (1991). The effects of framing, problem variations, and providing rationale on choice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 517–522

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • http://www.niaid.nih.gov/about/organization/dmid/documents/condomreport.pdf.

  • O’Connor, A. M., Boyd, N. F., Tritchler, D. L., Kriukov, Y., Sutherland, H., & Till, J. E. (1985). Eliciting preferences for alternative cancer drug treatments: The influence of framing, medium, and rater variables. Medical Decision Making, 5, 453–463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, A. M., Pennie, R. A., & Dales, R. E. (1996). Framing effects on expectations, decisions, and side effects experienced: The case of influenza immunization [published erratum appears in Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 1997;50:747−8]. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 49, 1271–1276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, D. J., & Jensen, J. D. (2007). The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease prevention behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Health Communication, 12, 623–644.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, D. J., & Jensen, J. D. (2009). The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease detection behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Communication, 59, 296–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palella, F. J., Deloria-Knoll, M., Chmiel, J. S., Moorman, A. C., Wood, K. C., Greenberg, A., et al. (2003). Survival benefit of initiating antiretroviral therapy in HIV-infected. Annals of Internal Medicine, 138, 620–626.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paling, J. (2003). Strategies to help patients understand risks. British Medical Journal, 327, 745–748.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E., & Levin, I. P. (2008). Dissecting the risky-choice framing effect: Numeracy as an individual-difference factor in weighting risky and riskless options. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 435–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E., Västfjäll, D., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Mazzocco, K., & Dickert, S. (2006). Numeracy and decision making. Psychological Science, 17, 407–413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (2007). The importance of mathematics in health and human judgment: Numeracy, risk communication, and medical decision making. Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 147–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (2008). Numeracy, ratio bias, and denominator neglect in judgments of risk and probability. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 89–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, V. R., Nelson, W. L., Han, P., & Dieckmann, N. F. (2009). How numeracy influences risk comprehension and medical decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 943–973.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A. J., Bartels, R. D., Wlaschin, J., & Salovey, P. (2006). The strategic use of gain- and loss-framed messages to promote healthy behavior: How theory can inform practice. Journal of Communication, 56, S202–S220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A. J., Martino, S. C., Bedell, B. T., Detweiler, J. B., & Salovey, P. (1999). The systematic influence of gain- and loss-framed messages on interest in and use of different types of health behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1355–1369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A. J., Martino, S. C., Bedell, B. T., Detweiler, J. B., & Salovey, P. (2003a). The systematic influence of gain- and loss-framed messages on interest in and use of different types of health behavior. In P. Salovey & A. J. Rothman (Eds.), Social psychology of health (pp. 286–299). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A. J., & Salovey, P. (1997). Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behaviour: The role of message framing. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 3–19.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A. J., Salovey, P., Antone, C., Keough, K., & Martin, C. (1993). The influence of message framing on health behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 408–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A., Stark, E. N., & Salovey, P. (2003b). Using message framing to promote healthful behaviors: A guide to best practices. In J. A. Trafton & W. P. Gordon (Eds.), Best practices in the behavioral management of chronic disease (pp. 31–45). Los Altos: Institute for Brain Potential.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salovey, P., & Wegener, D. T. (2003). Communicating about health: Message framing, persuasion and health behavior. In J. Suls & K. Wallston (Eds.), Social psychological foundations of health and illness (pp. 54–81). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Salovey, P., & Williams-Piehota, P. (2004). Field experiments in social psychology: Message framing and the promotion of health protective behaviors. American Behavioral Scientist, 47, 488–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, L. M., Woloshin, S., Black, W. C., & Welch, H. G. (1997). The role of numeracy in understanding the benefit of screening mammography. Annals of Internal Medicine, 127, 966–972.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, L. M., Woloshin, S., & Welch, G. (2005). Can patients interpret health information? An assessment of the medical data interpretation test. Medical Decision Making, 25, 290–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, D. K., Updegraff, J. A., & Mann, T. (2008). Improving oral health behavior. Journal of the American Dental Association, 139, 1382–1387.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shlay, J. C., McClung, M. W., Patnaik, J. L., & Douglas, J. M. (2004). Comparison of sexually transmitted disease prevalence by reported condom use: Errors among consistent condom users seen at an urban sexually transmitted disease clinic. Sexually Transmitted Diseases, 31, 526–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sieck, W., & Yates, J. F. (1997). Exposition effects on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70, 207–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siminoff, L. A., & Fetting, J. H. (1989). Effects of outcome framing on treatment decisions in the real world: Impact of framing on adjuvant breast cancer decisions. Medical Decision Making, 9, 262–271.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, A. F., Fagley, N. S., & Halleran, J. G. (2004). Decision framing: Moderating effects of individual differences and cognitive processing. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17, 77–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 290–312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual differences in framing and conjunction effects. Thinking and Reasoning, 4, 289–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffen, V. J., Sternberg, L., Teegarden, L. A., & Shepherd, K. (1994). Practice and persuasive frame: Effects on beliefs, intention, and performance of a cancer self-examination. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 897–925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toll, B. A., Martino, S., Latimer, A., Salovey, P., O’Malley, S., Calin-Menter, S., et al. (2010). Randomized trial: Quitline specialist training in grain-framed vs standard-care messages for smoking cessation. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 102, 96–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toll, B. A., O’Malley, S. S., Katulak, N. A., Wu, R., Dubin, J. A., Latimer, A., et al. (2007). Comparing gain- and loss-framed messages for smoking cessation with sustained-release brupropion: A randomized controlled trial. British Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 659–681.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiber, F. A. (1986). A comparison of positive and negative consequences approaches upon car restraint usage. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 11, 15–24.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tykocinski, O. E., Higgins, T., & Chaiken, S. (1994). Message framing, self-discrepancies, and yielding to persuasive messages: The motivational significance of psychological situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 107–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vidanapathirana, J., Abramson, M. J., Forbes, A., & Fairley, C. (2005). Mass media interventions for promoting HIV testing. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 3, Art. No.: CD004775.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walensky, R. P., Freedberg, K. A., Weinstein, M. C., & Paltiel, A. D. (2007). Cost-effectiveness of HIV testing and treatment in the United States. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 45, S248–S254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, E. U., & Johnson, E. J. (2006). Constructing preferences from memories. In S. Lichtenstein & P. Slovic (Eds.), The construction of preferences (pp. 397–410). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, E. U., Johnson, E. J., Milch, K. F., Chang, H., Brodscholl, J. C., & Goldstein, D. G. (2007). Asymmetric discounting in intertemporal choice: A query theory account. Psychological Science, 18, 516–523.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstock, H., Berman, S., & Cates, W. (2004). Sexually transmitted diseases among American youth: Incidence and prevalence estimates. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 36, 6–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weller, S. C., & Davis-Beaty, K. (2002). Condom effectiveness in reducing heterosexual HIV transmission. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 1, Art. No.: CD003255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, T., Clarke, V., & Borland, R. (2001). Effects of message framing on breast-cancer-related beliefs and behaviors: The role of mediating factors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 925–950.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. K., Kaplan, R. M., & Schneiderman, L. J. (1987). Framing of decisions and selections of alternatives in health care. Social Behavior, 2, 51–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. K., Purdon, S. E., & Wallston, K. A. (1988). Compliance to health recommendations: A theoretical overview of message framing. Health Education Research, 3, 161–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woloshin, S., Schwartz, L. M., Black, W. C., & Welch, H. G. (1999). Women’s perceptions of breast cancer risk: How you ask matters. Medical Decision Making, 19, 221–229.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization, Europe. (2005). The health of children and adolescents in Europe: Fact sheet EURO/06/05. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wortley, P. M., Chu, S. T., Diaz, T., Ward, J. W., Doyle, B., Davidson, A. J., et al. (1995). HIV testing patterns: Where, why, and when were persons with AIDS tested for HIV? AIDS, 9, 487–492.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rocio Garcia-Retamero Ph.D. .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Garcia-Retamero, R., Cokely, E.T., Galesic, M. (2012). Reducing the Effect of Framed Messages About Health. In: Garcia-Retamero, R., Galesic, M. (eds) Transparent Communication of Health Risks. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-4358-2_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-4358-2_11

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4614-4357-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4614-4358-2

  • eBook Packages: MedicineMedicine (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics