Skip to main content

Patterns of Influence Among Phenomenological Awareness, Social Experience and Conceptual Knowledge

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Children’s Discovery of the Active Mind

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Psychology ((BRIEFSCD))

  • 846 Accesses

Abstract

Proposals concerning patterns of influence among children’s developing conceptual knowledge of cognition, children’s phenomenological awareness of their own cognitive functioning, and social experience are further elaborated. Relations of reciprocal influence between conceptual knowledge and cognitive monitoring, conceptual knowledge and social experience, and social experience and cognitive monitoring are described. In addition, possible developmental mechanisms underlying the acquisition of knowledge about cognitive activities are considered, focusing on the issue of domain-specific modules versus domain-general learning processes. As children learn about cognition, general learning processes, particularly pattern recognition and executive function, may help them to integrate their mental state concepts with information available through first-person phenomenological experience and social experience.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 34.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 49.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81, 1641–1660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind?: contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11, 73–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., & Brown, J. (1993). Early conversations about causality: content, pragmatics, and developmental change. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 107–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1981). Cognitive monitoring. In W. P. Dickson (Ed.), Children’s oral communication skills (pp. 35–60). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foley, M., Ratner, H. H., & House, A. T. (2002). Anticipation and source-monitoring errors: children’s memory for collaborative activities. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3, 385–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghatala, E. S., Levin, J. R., Pressley, M., & Goodwin, D. (1986). A componential analysis of the effects of derived and supplied strategy-utility information on children’s strategy selections. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 76–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1997). “Pretend you didn’t know”: preschoolers’ talk about mental states in pretend play. Cognitive Development, 12, 477–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, N. (1983). Consciousness regained: Chapters in the development of mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, N. (1986). The inner eye. London: Faber and Faber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1998). Illusions of knowing: The link between knowledge and metaknowledge. In V. Y. Yzerbyt, G. Lories, & B. Dardenne (Eds.), Metacognition: Cognitive and social dimensions (pp. 16–34). London: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBy, and agency: Core architecture and domain specficity. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 119–148). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lillard, A. (1999). Developing a cultural theory of mind: the CIAO approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 57–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (2002). Consciousness recovered: Psychological functions and origins of conscious thought. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGivern, J. E., Levin, J. R., Pressley, M., & Ghatala, E. S. (1990). A developmental study of memory monitoring and strategy selection. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 15, 103–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. (2006). The development of commonsense psychology. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., Kruglanski, A. W., & Jost, J. T. (1998). Knowing thyself and others: Progress in metacognitive social psychology. In V. Y. Yzerbyt, G. Lories, & D. Benoit (Eds.), Metacognition: Cognitive and social dimensions (pp. 69–89). London: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palincsar, A. S., & Magnusson, S. J. (2001). The interplay of first-hand and second-hand investigations to model and support the development of scientific knowledge and reasoning. In S. M. Carver & D. Klahr (Eds.), Cognition and instruction: Twenty-five years of progress. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pillow, B. H., & Anderson, K. L. (2006). Children’s awareness of their own certainty and understanding of deduction and guessing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 823–849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., Ross, K. A., Levin, J. R., & Ghatala, E. S. (1984). The role of strategy-utility knowledge in children’s decision making. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 491–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratner, H. H., Foley, M. A., & Gimpert, N. (2002). The role of collaborative planning in children’s source monitoring errors and learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 44–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R. (2003). Statistical learning: mechanisms and constraints. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 110–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salonen, P., Vauras, M., & Efklides, A. (2005). Social interaction: what can it tell us about metacognition and coregulation in learning? European Psychologist, 10, 199–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., Fabricius, W. V., & Noyes, C. R. (1996). Developing organization of mental verbs: evidence for the development of a constructivist theory of mind in middle childhood. Cognitive Development, 11, 265–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bradford H. Pillow .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Pillow, B.H. (2012). Patterns of Influence Among Phenomenological Awareness, Social Experience and Conceptual Knowledge. In: Children’s Discovery of the Active Mind. SpringerBriefs in Psychology(). Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2248-8_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics