Knowledge and Understanding

Chapter
Part of the SpringerBriefs in Biology book series (BRIEFSBIOL, volume 1)

Abstract

One of the most remarkable things the brain does is transform the raw data of experience into the informational structures that provide a sense of meaning and understanding to our lives. Knowledge is comprised of organized sets of meaningful information that are encoded in the central nervous system, some based on personal experience and some on what others have passed on to us. Understanding is a special form of knowledge that involves appreciating how interacting entities affect each other. Rule-based reasoning involves the sequential processing of discrete bits of information according to strict algorithmic rules, while pattern-based reasoning involves searching for correspondences and contrasts between perceived patterns and reference ones. We organize and rearrange information to build models that reflect our understanding of how individual facts and inferences are linked and interact. We have no way of being certain that the knowledge we accrue is correct, however, since our information-processing systems are not good truth-detectors.

Keywords

Platinum Bark Arena Kelly 

References

  1. Bruner J (1986) Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  2. Burton RA (2008) On Being Certain: Believing You are Right Even When You Are Not. St. Martin’s Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  3. Byrne RMJ (2005) The Rational Imagination: How People Create Alternatives to Reality. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  4. Craik KJW (1943) The Nature of Explanation. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  5. Franklin J (2001) The Science of Conjecture: Evidence and Probability before Pascal. Johns Hopkins University Press, BaltimoreGoogle Scholar
  6. Gentner G, Holyoak K, Kokinov B (2001) The Analogical Mind. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  7. Hofstadter DR (2007) I Am a Strange Loop. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  8. Holland JH (1992) Complex adaptive systems. Daedalus, 121 (Winter): 17–30Google Scholar
  9. Hong FT (2005) A multi-disciplinary survey of biocomputing: 2. Systems and evolutionary levels, and technical applications. In VB Bajic, TT Wee (Eds), Information Processing and Living Systems. Imperial College Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  10. Johnson-Laird PN (1996) Mental Models. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  11. Johnson-Laird PN (2006) How We Reason. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  12. Kelly GA (1955) The Psychology of Personal Constructs, Vol 1. WW Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  13. Piaget J, Inhelder B (1969) The Psychology of the Child. Trans. H Weaver. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  14. Popper KR (1959) The Logic of Scientific Discovery, 2nd edn. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  15. Rivett P (1972) Principles of Model Building. John Wiley & Sons, LondonGoogle Scholar
  16. Suppe F (1977) The Structure of Scientific Theories. University of Illinois Press, UrbanaGoogle Scholar
  17. Taleb NN (2007) The Black Swan: The Impact of the Highly Improbable. Random House, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  18. Whitehorn JC (1963) Education for uncertainty. Perspect Biol Med. 7: 118–123PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Wiener N (1993) Invention: The Care and Feeding of Ideas. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of South FloridaFloridaUSA

Personalised recommendations