Skip to main content

Implications of Cognitive Theory for Knowledge Acquisition

  • Chapter

Abstract

Knowledge acquisition for expert systems is a subclass of any instructional situation whereby knowledge must be externalized from a human expert and transferred to one or more “systems.” These systems have historically been other people, as in education and training. However, with the advent of artificial intelligence, the systems are more frequently becoming computers. There are many issues that cut across all types of knowledge acquisition. However, two key questions are, (a) What is the nature of the knowledge and/or skills used by an expert? and (b) What are the implications of the nature of expertise for methods of transferring the knowledge or skill?

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Akin, O. (1980). Models of architectural knowledge. London: Pion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altman, R. B., & Buchanan, B. G. (1987). Partial compilation of strategic knowledge. Proceed- ings of the AAAI Sixth National Conference on Artificial Intelligence (pp. 399–404). Los Altos, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1976). Language memory,and thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1985). Cognitive psychology and its implications. New York: W.H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1987a). Skill acquisition: Compilation of week-method problem solutions. Psychological Review 94, 192–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1987b). Production systems, learning, and tutoring. In D. Klahr, P. Langley, & R. Neches (Eds.), Production system models of learning and development (pp. 437–458). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1989). The analogical origins of errors in problem solving. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processes: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 343–371). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arciszewski, T., Mustafa, M., & Ziarko, W. (1987). A methodology of design knowledge acquisition for use in learning expert systems. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 27, 23–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, D. C., & Broadbent, D. E. (1984). On the relationship between task performance and associated verbalizable knowledge. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 36A, 209–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaxton, T. A. (1989). Investigating dissociations among memory measures: Support for a transfer-appropriate processing framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 15, 657–668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boose, J. H. (1986). Expertise transfer for expert system design. New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boose, J. H. (1988). Uses of repertory grid-centred knowledge acquisition tools for knowledge-based systems. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 29, 287–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E., & Aston, B. (1978). Human control of a simulated economic system. Ergonomics 21, 1035–1043.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, D. N., & Baddeley, A. D. (1976). What can amnesic patients learn? Neurophsychologia, 14, 111–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butters, N. (1987, February). Procedural learning in dementia: A double dissociation between Alzheimer and Huntington’s disease patients on verbal priming and motor skill learning. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, T. H., McCauley, C., Sperber, R. D., & Parmalee, C. M. (1982). Words, pictures, and printing: On semantic activation, conscious identification, and the automaticity of information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8, 757–777.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charness, N., & Campbell, J. I. D. (1988). Acquiring skill at mental calculation in adulthood: A task decomposition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 117, 115–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G. (1983). Spatial representations of taxi drivers. In D. R. Rogers & J. H. Sloboda (Eds.), Acquisition of symbolic skills (pp. 391–405). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (Eds.). (1988). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem solving. In R. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 1, pp. 17–76). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clancey, W. J. (1983). The epistemology of a rule-based expert system: A framework for explanation. Artificial Intelligence 20, 215–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, R., & Morton, J. (1983). Cross-modality facilitation in tachistoscopic word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 35A, 79–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A., Ivry, R., & Keele, S. W. (1988). Attention and structure in sequence learning (Technical Report No. 88–5). Institute of Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, N. J. (1984). Preserved learning capacity in amnesia: Evidence for multiple memory systems. In L. R. Squire & N. Butters (Eds.), Neuro-psychology of memory (pp. 83–103). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of “knowing how” and “knowing that.” Science 210, 207–209.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review 82, 407–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corkin, S. (1968). Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial temporal excision. Neuropsychologia, 6, 255–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G. (1989). Modularity and dissociations in memory systems. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 271–294). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dannenbring, G. L., & Briand, K. (1982). Semantic priming and the word repetition effect in a lexical decision task. Canadian Journal of Psychology 36, 435–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Day, B. L., & Marsden, C. D. (1982). Two strategies for learning a visually guided motor task. Perceptual and Motor Skills 55, 1003–1016.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • deJong, T., & Ferguson-Hessler, M. G. M. (1986). Cognitive structures of good and poor novice problem solvers in physics. Journal of Educational Psychology 78, 279–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diederich, J., Ruhmann, I., & May, M. (1987). KRITON: A knowledge-acquisition tool for expert systems. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 26, 29–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dillon, R. F., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.). (1986). Cognition and instruction. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, D. E., & Schwartz, B. J. (1979). Chunking in recall of symbolic drawings. Memory and Cognition, 7, 149–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1984). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, F. J., & Thorn, W. A. F. (1966). Two types of posthypnotic amnesia: Recall amnesia and source amnesia. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,14, 162–179.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franks, I. M. (1982). Rehearsal and learning of embedded movement sequences during a tracking task. Perceptual and Motor Skills 55, 615–622.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gagne, E. D. (1985). The cognitive psychology of school learning. Boston, MA: Little, Brown & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagne, R. M., & Briggs, L. J. (1979). Principles of instructional design. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. R. (1988). Expertise in typewriting. In M. T. H. Chi, R. Glaser, & M. J. Farr (Eds.), The nature of expertise (pp. 1–21). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, R. (1984). Education and thinking: The role of knowledge. American Psychologist 39, 93–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, R. (1989). Expertise and learning: How do we think about instructional processes now that we have discovered knowledge structures? In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 269–282). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glisky, E. L., Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (1986). Computer learning by memory-impaired patients: Acquisition and retention of complex knowledge. Neuropsychologia 24, 313–328.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, S. E. (1991). Front-end analysis for expert system design. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 35th Annual Meeting (pp. 278–282). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, S. E., & Gill, R. T. (1989). Question probes: A structured method for eliciting declarative knowledge. AI Applications in Natural Resource Management, 3, 13–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, S. E., & Gill, R. T. (in press). Knowledge acquisition with question probes and conceptual graph structures. In T. Lauer, E. Peacock, & A. Graesser (Eds.), Questions and information systems. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., & Mandler, G. (1984). Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23, 553–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., Mandler, G., & Hayden, P. (1982). Simulating amnesic symptoms in normal subjects. Science 218, 1243–1244.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1987). Selective effects of interference on implicit and explicit memory for new associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 13, 45–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1985). Priming across modalities and priming across category levels: Extending the domain of preserved function in amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 11, 385–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., Squire, L. R., & Mandler, G. (1984). The information that amnesic patients do not forget. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 10, 164–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grover, M. D. (1983). A pragmatic knowledge acquisition methodology. International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence 83, 436–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, A. (1986). Knowledge acquisition for expert systems. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, N. E., & Broadbent, D. (1988). Two modes of learning for interactive tasks. Cognition 28, 249–276.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L. (1983). Perceptual enhancement: Persistent effects of an experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 9, 21–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between auto-biographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 110, 306–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, P. E. (1983). What kind of expert should a system be? The Journal of Medicine and Philosophy 8, 77–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg, R. T. (1982). Hypothesis recognition failure in conjunctive and disjunctive concept-identification tasks. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 19, 327–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg, R. T., Robbins, D. W., & Bourne, L. E. (1978). Memory for intratrial events in feature identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory,4, 256–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review 95, 163–182.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsner, K., Milech, D., & Standen, P. (1983). Common and modality-specific processes in the mental lexicon. Memory & Cognition,11, 621–630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, G. A., Calderwood, R., & Clinton-Cirocco, A. (1986). Rapid decision making on the fire ground. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 30th Annual Meeting (pp. 576–580). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langley, P., & Carbonell, J. G. (1984). Approaches to machine learning. Journal of the American Society for Information Science 35, 306–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J. H. (1979, December). Processing information for effective problem solving. Engineering Education, pp. 285–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewicki, P. (1985). Nonconscious biasing effects of single instances on subsequent judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48, 563–574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewicki, P. (1986). Processing information about covariations that cannot be articulated. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 12, 135–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewicki, P., Czyzewska, M., & Hoffman, H. (1987). Unconscious acquisition of complex procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 13, 523–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewicki, P., Hill, T., & Bizot, E. (1988). Acquisition of procedural knowledge about a pattern of stimuli that cannot be articulated. Cognitive Psychology 20, 24–37.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Light, L. L., & Singh, A. (1987). Implicit and explicit memory in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 13, 531–541.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Light, L. L., Singh, A., & Capps, J. L. (1986). Dissociation of memory and awareness in young and older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,8, 62–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1980). Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data. Cognitive Psychology 12, 523–553.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1988). Automaticity, resources, and memory: Theoretical controversies and practical implications. Human Factors 30, 583–598.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, A. T. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology 15, 238–300.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McKeithen, K. B., Reitman, J. S., Rueter, H. H., & Hirtle, S. C. (1981). Knowledge organization and skill differences in computer programmers. Cognitive Psychology 13, 307–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michalski, R. S., Carbonell, J. G., & Mitchell, T. M. (Eds.). (1986). Machine learning: An artificial intelligence approach (Vol. 2). Los Altos, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner, B., Corkin, S., & Teuber, H. L. (1968). Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14 year follow-up study of H. M. Neuropsychologia 6, 215–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, A. A. (1987). The use of alternative knowledge-acquisition procedures in the development of a knowledge-based media planning system. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 26, 399–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, D. B. (1989). How many memory systems? Evidence from aging. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 15, 31–49.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, D. B., & Brown, A. S. (1988). Persistent repetition priming in picture naming and its dissociation from recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 14, 213–222.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moscovitch, M. (1989). Confabulation and the frontal systems: Stretegic versus associated retrieval in neuropsychological theories of memory. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 133–161). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Musen, M. A., Fagan, L. M., Combs, D. M., & Shortliffe, E. H. (1987). Use of a domain model to drive an interactive knowledge-editing tool. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 26, 105–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, L., & Zola-Margan, S. (1984). Toward the understanding of infant memory: Contributions from animal neuropsychology. In M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Infant memory (pp. 145–172). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 106, 226–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neves, D. M., & Anderson, J. R. (1981). Knowledge compilation: Mechanisms for the automatization of cognitive skills. In J. A. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 57–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R. S., Perkins, D. N., & Smith, E. E. (1985). The teaching of thinking. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84, 231–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology 19, 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1975). Explorations in cognition. New York: W.H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porter, D. B. (1987). Classroom teaching, implicit learning and the deleterious effects of inappropriate explication. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 31st Annual Meeting (pp. 289–292). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, J. (1980). The human as a system component. In H. Smith & T. Green (Eds.), Human interaction with computers. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, J. (1983). Skills, rules, knowledge: Signals, signs, and symbols and other distinctions in human performance models. IEEE Transaction on Systems Man and Cybernetics 13, 257–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, J. (1986). Information processing and human-machine interaction: An approach to cognitive engineering. New York: Elsevier Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 855–863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1976). Implicit learning of synthetic languages: The role of instructional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory,2, 88–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S., & Lewis, S. (1977). Implicit learning: An analysis of the form and structure of a body of tacit knowledge. Cognition 5,333–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reif, F., & Heller, J. I. (1982). Knowledge structures and problem solving in physics. Educational Psychologist 17, 102–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L. B. (1981). Instructional psychology. Annual Review of Psychology 32, 659–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson-Klavehn, A., & Bjork, R. A. (1988). Measures of memory. Annual Review of Psychology 39, 475–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., & Blaxton, T. A. (1987). Effects of varying modality, surface features, and retention interval on priming in word fragment completion. Memory and Cognition 15, 379–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., Weldon, M. S., & Challis, B. H. (1989). Explaining dissociations between implicit and explicit measures of retention: A processing account. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 3–41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. San Francisco, CA: Hutchinson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanderson, P. M. (1989). Verbalizable knowledge and skilled task performance: Association, dissociation, and mental models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 15, 729–747.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scandura, J. M. (1977). Problem solving: A structural/process approach with instructional implications. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1985). Priming of old and new knowledge in amnesic patients and normal subjects. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 444, 41–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology 13, 501–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1989). On the relation between memory and consciousness: Dissociable interactions and conscious experience. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 355–389). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., & Graf, P. (1986). Effects of elaborative processing on implicit and explicit memory for new associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,Memory,and Cognition 12, 432–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Harbluk, J. L., & McLachlan, D. R. (1984). Retrieval without rocollection: An experimental analysis of source amnesia. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23, 593–611.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Detweiler, M. (1987). Connectionist/control architecture for working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 21 (pp. 53–119). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, T. J. (1989, December). Parables of neural networks. AI Expert,pp. 54–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Dumais, S. T. (1981). The development of automatism. In J. R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 111–140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P. (1986). Priming effects in amnesia: Evidence for a dissociable memory function. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 38A, 619–644.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1984). Paired-associate learning and priming effects in amnesia: A neuropsychological study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,113, 556–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1987). A neuropsychological study of fact learning and source amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 13, 464–474.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1988). Longterm memory in amnesia: Cued recall, recognition memory, and confidence ratings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 14, 763–770.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Singley, M. K., & Anderson, J. R. (1988). A keystroke analysis of learning and transfer in text editing. Human-Computer Interactions, 3, 223–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singley, M. K., & Anderson, J. R. (1989). The transfer of cognitive skill. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. A., Hayman, C. A., Ohta, N., Law, J., & Tulving, E. (1988). Forgetting in primed fragment completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 14, 223–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R., & Branscombe, N. R. (1988). Category accessibility as implicit memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 24, 490–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. M. (1987). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R., & Cohen, N. J. (1984). Human memory and amnesia. In J. McGaugh, G. Lynch, & N. Weinberger (Eds.), Proceedings of the conference on the neurobiology of learning and memory (pp. 3–64). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stadler, M. A. (1989). On learning complex procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 15, 1061–1069.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stauffer, L. (1989). Eliciting and analyzing data about the engineering-design process. Proceedings of the NSF Design Theory and Methodology Conference, University of Massachussetts, Amherst, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steels, L. (1987). Second generation expert systems. In M. A. Bramer (Ed.), Research and development in expert systems, Vol. 3 (pp. 175–183). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist 40, 385–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., Schacter, D. L., & Stark, H. A. (1982). Priming effects in word-fragment completion are independent of recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,Memory and Cognition 8,336–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldron, M. B., & Waldron, K. L. (1988). A time sequence study of a complex mechanical system design. Design Studies 9, 95–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1970). Amnesia: Consolidation or retrieval? Nature 228, 628–630.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1974). The effect of prior learning on subsequent retention in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia 12, 419–428.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1982). Amnesia: A disconnection syndrome? Neuropsychologia 20, 233–248.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, M. S., & Roediger, H. L. (1987). Altering retrieval demands reverses the picture superiority effect. Memory and Cognition 15, 269–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D. B., Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1989). On the development of procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,Memory and Cognition,15, 1047–1060.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winnick, W. A., & Daniel, S. A. (1970). Two kinds of response priming in tachistoscopic word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology,84, 74–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witten, I. H., & McDonald, B. A. (1988). Using concept learning for knowledge acquisition. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 29, 171–196.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 Springer-Verlag New York, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gordon, S.E. (1992). Implications of Cognitive Theory for Knowledge Acquisition. In: Hoffman, R.R. (eds) The Psychology of Expertise. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9733-5_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9733-5_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-9735-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4613-9733-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics