Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Cognitive Development ((2116))

Abstract

The purpose of this chapter is to explore ways in which we can characterize conceptual development. We will briefly examine existing approaches to studying the topic, point to their strengths and weaknesses, propose an alternative approach, and illustrate the alternative with examples involving children’s concepts of numbers and of life. The recurring theme will be that conceptual understanding is multifaceted, and that our approaches to studying it must be consistent with this fact.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aiken, L. R., & Williams E. N. Three variables related to reaction time to compare digits. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1968, 27, 199–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. Language, memory, and thought. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. Arguments concerning representations for mental imagery. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 249–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashcraft, M. H. The development of mental arithmetic: A chronometric approach. Developmental Review, 1982, 2, 213–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashcraft, M. H., & Battaglia, J. Cognitive arithmetic: Evidence for retrieval and decision processes in mental addition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1978, 4, 527–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banks, W. P. Encoding and processing of symbolic information in comparative judgments. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 1977, 11, 101–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banks, W. P., Fujii, M., & Kayra-Stuart, F. Semantic congruity effects in comparative judgments of magnitude of digits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1976, 2, 435–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilin, H. Cognitive capacities of young children: A replication. Science, 1968, 162, 920–921.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bever, T. G., Mehler, J., & Epstein, J. What children do in spite of what they know. Science, 1968, 162, 921–924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M. D. S. The ontogeny of certain logical operations: Piaget’s formulation examined by nonverbal methods. Psychological Monographs, 1959, 73, no. 5 (Whole No. 475).

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. Piaget’s theory of intelligence. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. The origins of the number concept. New York: Praeger, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L. The construction of temporal succession by pre-operational children. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (Vol. 10). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. Beyond the information given: Studies in the psychology of knowing. New York: Norton, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E. Perception and understanding in young children. New York: Basic Books, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fairbank, B. A. Experiments on the temporal aspects of number perception. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Arizona, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. Stage-related properties of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 1971, 2, 421–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. C., Richards, J., & Briars, D. J. The acquisition and elaboration of the number word sequence. In C. Brainerd (Ed.), Progress in cognitive development (Vol 1). New York: Springer-Verlag, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R. The nature and development of early number concepts. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 1972, 7, 115–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. F. The child’s understanding of number. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, H. Children’s arithmetic: The learning process. New York: Van Nostrand, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groen, G. J., & Parkman, J. M. A chronometric analysis of simple addition. Psychological Review, 1972, 79, 329–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebbeler, K. The development of children’s problem-solving skills in addition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cornell University, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, I. Children’s conception of physical causality: A critical summary. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1943, 63, 71–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, I., & Lee, H. W. Experimental analysis of child animism. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1945, 66, 69–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ilg, F., & Ames, L. B., Developmental trends in arithmetic. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1951, 79, 3–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. C. Hierarchical clustering schemes. Psychometrika, 1967, 32, 241–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., & Wallace, J. G. Cognitive development: An information-processing view. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klingberg, G. The distinction between living and not living among 7–10-year-old children, with some remarks concerning the so-called animism controversy. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1957, 90, 227–238.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klingensmith, S. W. Child animism: What the child means by “alive.” Child Development, 1953, 24, 51–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knight, F. B., & Behrens, M. S. The learning of the 100 addition combinations and the 100 subtraction combinations. New York: Longmans, Green, 1928.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. Imagery and cognitive development: A teleological approach. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1978

    Google Scholar 

  • Laurendeau, M., & Pinard, A. Causal thinking in the child. New York: International Universities Press, 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehman, H. Introduction to the philosophy of mathematics. Totowa, N.J.: Row-man & Littlefield, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S. A. Nonverbal assessment of conservation of number. Child Development, 1976, 47, 722–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, R. S., & Bayer, R. H. Mental comparison and the symbolic distance effect. Cognitive Psychology, 1976, 8, 228–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, R. S., & Dumais, S. T. Mental comparison. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 1978, 12, 117–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. The time required for judgments of numerical inequality. Nature (London), 1961, 215, 1519–1520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. A note on process-structure distinctions in developmental psychology. In S. Farnham-Diggory (Ed.), Information processing in children. New York: Academic Press, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parkman, J. M. Temporal aspects of digit and letter inequality judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1971, 91, 191–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. The child’s concept of number. New York: Norton, 1952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. Intellectual evolution from adolescence to adulthood. Human Development, 1972, 15, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. The child’s conception of the world. St. Albans, Great Britain: Paladin, 1973.(Originally published in 1929.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollio, H. R., & Whitacre, J. Some observations on the use of natural numbers by preschool children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1970, 30, 167–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, D. D. Children’s concept learning: The child’s concept of life. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Carnegie-Mellon University, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothenberg, B. B., & Courtney, R. G. Conservation of number in very young children. Development Psychology, 1969, 1, 493–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, R. W. Studies in animism, II: The development of animism. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1940, 56, 353–366. (a)

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, R. W. Studies in animism, IV: An investigation of the concepts allied to animism. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1940, 57, 83–91. (b)

    Google Scholar 

  • Sekuler, R., Armstrong, R., & Rubin, E. Processing numerical information: A choice time analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1971, 90, 75–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sekuler, R., & Mierkiewicz, D. Children’s judgments of numerical inequality. Child Development, 1977, 48, 630–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. Three aspects of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 1976, 8, 481–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. The origins of scientific reasoning. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. Developmental sequences within and between concepts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1981, 46, no. 2 (Whole No. 189).

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Robinson, M. The development of numerical understandings. In H. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. On the development of the processor. In S. Farnham-Diggory (Ed.), Information processing in children. New York: Academic Press, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Svenson, O. Analysis of time required by children for simple additions. Acta Psychologica, 1975, 39, 289–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trabasso, T. The role of memory as a system in making transitive inferences. In R. V. Kail, Jr., & J. W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoshimura, T. Strategies for addition among young children. Paper presented at the 16th annual convention of the Japanese Association of Educational Psychology, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1983 Springer-Verlag, New York, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Siegler, R.S., Richards, D.D. (1983). The Development of Two Concepts. In: Brainerd, C.J. (eds) Recent Advances in Cognitive-Developmental Theory. Springer Series in Cognitive Development. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9490-7_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9490-7_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-9492-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4613-9490-7

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics