Skip to main content

The Role of a Global Mechanism in Developmental Change in Speed of Processing

  • Chapter
Emerging Themes in Cognitive Development

Abstract

Improvements during childhood and adolescence in performance on speeded tasks are ubiquitous. For tasks such as scanning the contents of short-term memory, retrieving names from long-term memory, verifying the truth of arithmetic statements, and reasoning analogically, the results are the same: Adults respond substantially more rapidly than do children (e.g., Ashcraft & Fierman, 1982; Bisanz, Danner, & Resnick, 1979; Sternberg & Rifkin, 1979). It has been proposed that these results reflect some global mechanism (i.e., one not specific to a particular task) that limits the speed with which all processes can be executed (Hale, 1990; Kail, 1988). As this mechanism changes with age, all processes are executed more rapidly.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anderson, J.R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashcraft, M.A., & Fierman, B.A. (1982). Mental addition in third-, fourth, and sixth graders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 216–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birren, J.E. (1965). Age changes in speed of behavior: Its central nature and physiological correlates. In A.T. Welford & J.E. Birren (Eds.), Behavior, aging and the nervous system (pp. 191–216). Springfield, IL: Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisanz, J. (1979). Processes and strategies in children’s solutions of geometric analogies. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisanz, J., Danner, F., & Resnick, L.B. (1979). Changes with age in measures of processing efficiency. Child Development, 50, 132–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D.F., & Harnishfeger, K.K. (1990). The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 10, 48–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, N., & Smith, G.A. (1989). Developmental changes in processing speed: Influence of speed-accuracy regulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 298–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J.I.D., & Graham, D.J. (1985). Mental multiplication skill: Structure, process and acquisition. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 338–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, M., Burne, B., & Kirsner, K. (1985). Autobiographical memory and perceptual learning: A developmental study using picture recognition, naming latency, and perceptual identification. Memory & Cognition, 13, 273–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development, birth to adulthood. Orlando: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cerella, J. (1985). Information processing rates in the elderly. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 67–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M.T.H. (1977). Age differences in the speed of processing: A critique. Developmental Psychology, 13, 543–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, J.D., & Corsale, K. (1977). The effects of mode and rate of presentation on evaluative encoding in children’s memory. Child Development, 48, 46–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, R. (1972). Simple reaction time in children: Effects of incentive, incentive-shift and other training variables. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 13, 540–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodenough, F.L. (1935). The development of the reactive process from early childhood to maturity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 431–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hale, S. (1990). A global developmental trend in cognitive processing speed in children. Child Development, 61, 653–663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howe, M.L., & Rabinowitz, F.M. (1990). Resource panacea? Or just another day in the developmental forest. Developmental Review, 10, 125–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1986a). The impact of extended practice on rate of mental rotation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 378–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1986b). Sources of age differences in speed of processing. Child Development, 57, 969–987.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1988). Developmental functions for speeds of cognitive processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 45, 339–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1991a). Controlled and automatic processing during mental rotation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 337–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1991b). Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 490–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1991c). Processing time declines exponentially during childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 27, 259–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., & Park, Y. (1990). Impact of practice on speed of mental rotation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 227–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. & Park, Y. (in press). A global mechanism is implicated in developmental change in speed of processing. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., & Schroll, J.T. (1974). Evaluative and taxonomic encoding in children’s memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 426–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, B. (1979). Sequential predictablity effects on initiation time and movement time for adults and children. Journal of Motor Behavior, 11, 71–79.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsner, K. (1972). Developmental changes in short-term recognition memory. British Journal of Psychology, 63, 109–117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D. (1989). Information-processing approaches. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development (Vol. 6, pp. 133–185). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klorman, R. (1975). Contingent negative variation and cardiac deceleration in a long preparatory interval: a developmental study. Psychophysiology, 12, 609–617.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, L.E. (1973). Effect of letter-pair frequency and orientation of speed of “same”-”different” judgments by children and adults. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 2, 431–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawler, K.A., Obrist, P.A., & Lawler, J.E. (1976). Cardiac and somatic response pattern during a reaction-time task in children and adults. Psychophysiology, 13, 448–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, R., & Calfee, R.C. (1971). Visual search through word lists as a function of grade level, reading ability and target repetition. Perception & Psychophysics, 10, 169–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L.W., & Smith, G.A. (1990). The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97, 475–487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naus, M.J., Ornstein, P.A., & Aivano, S. (1977). Developmental changes in memory: The effects of processing time and rehearsal instructions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 237–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Rosenbloom, P.S. (1981). Mechanisms of skill acquisition and the law of practice. In J.R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 1–55). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C.E., Suci, G.J., & Tannenbaum, P.H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peretti, P.O. (1969). Cross-sex and cross-educational level performance in a color-word interference task. Psychonomic Science, 16, 321–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pezdek, K., & Miceli, L. (1982). Life-span differences in semantic integration of pictures and sentences in memory. Child Development, 51, 720–729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, C. (1983). Factors affecting developmental changes in the speed of processing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 509–528.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salmoni, A.W., & Bascoe, C. (1979). Fitt’s reciprocal tapping task: a development study. In G.C. Roberts & K.M. Newell (Eds.), The Psychology of Motor Behavior and Sport (pp. 288–294). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T.A. (1988). The role of processing resources in cognitive aging. In M.L. Howe & C.J. Brainerd (Eds.), Cognitive development in adulthood (pp. 185–239). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T.A. (1990). Working memory as a processing resource in cognitive aging. Developmental Review, 10, 101–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T.A., & Kail, R. (1983). Memory development throughout the life span: The role of processing rate. In P.B. Baltes & O.G. Brim (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 5, pp. 89–116). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T.A., & Somberg, B.L. (1982). Isolating the age difference in speeded performance. Journal of Gerontology, 37, 59–63.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, S.J., Begy, G., & Chen, C.C. (1975–76). Comparison of word recognition speed and strategies of less skilled and more highly skilled readers. Reading Research Quartery, 11, 72–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, C.H., Krampitz, J.B., Northam, C.C., & Ashby, A.A. (1982). Information processing in coincident timing taks: A developmental perspective. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 8, 73–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R.M., & Dumais, S.T. (1981). The development of automatism. In J.R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 111–140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R.S. (1976). Three aspects of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 481–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R.S. (1987). The perils of averaging data over strategies: An example from children’s addition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 250–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R.J., & Rifkin, B. (1979). The development of analogical reasoning processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 195–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Surwillo, W.W., & Titus, T.G. (1976). Reaction time and the psychological refractory period in children and adults. Developmental Psychobiology, 9, 517–527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Svenson, O., & Sjoberg, K. (1983). Speeds of subitizing and counting processes in different age groups. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 142, 203–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, C.D. (1974). Temporal limits of human information processing: A developmental study. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 739–755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, C.D., & Benel, D.C.R. (1982). The development of time-sharing skills. In J. A.S. Kelso & J.E. Clark (Eds.), The development of movement control and coordination (pp. 253–272). New York/London: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, D.D. (1970). Encoding categories of words: An empirical approach to meaning. Psychological Review, 77, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. (1985). Age differences on a coincident anticipation task: influence of stereotypic or “preferred” movement speed. Journal of Motor Behavior, 17, 389–410.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Young, J.M., Palef, S.R., & Logan, G.D. (1980). The role of mental rotation in letter processing by children and adults. Canadial Journal of Psychology, 34, 265–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1993 Springer-Verlag New York Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kail, R. (1993). The Role of a Global Mechanism in Developmental Change in Speed of Processing. In: Howe, M.L., Pasnak, R. (eds) Emerging Themes in Cognitive Development. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9220-0_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9220-0_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-9222-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4613-9220-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics