The Use of Auditory Evoked Responses Recorded from Newborn Infants to Predict Language Skills

  • Dennis L. Molfese
Part of the Advances in Child Neuropsychology book series (CHILDNEUROPSYCH, volume 1)

Abstract

Over the past two decades, an increasing number of investigations reported that various behavioral and medical measures are useful in characterizing the development of normal infants as well as those infants who may be at risk for a variety of learning disabilities (see V. Molfese, 1989, for a review and evaluation of the effectiveness of these measures). Although such research reports appear promising, the correlations reported and the percentage of variance accounted for are typically low. At best, 50% of the variance has been accounted for in predictive studies utilizing birth measures as predictors of outcomes in the preschool period (Molfese & Molfese, 1985). In an effort to predict more accurately later developmental outcomes, researchers have been turning toward other measures such as the brain stem evoked response and the long latency evoked potential response that might converge with these traditional behavioral assessment measures in order to build better predictive models. Studies utilizing such evoked brain responses have for some time been viewed as providing new information concerning the structural integrity of the brain, insights into brain functioning, and the development of hemispheric involvement in cognitive and language abilities (Callaway, Tueting, & Koslow, 1978).

Keywords

Income Lester 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Als, H., Tronick, E., Lester, B., & Brazelton, T. (1977). The Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (BNAS). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 5, 215–231.Google Scholar
  2. Barden, T., & Peltzman, P. (1980). Newborn brain stem auditory evoked responses and perinatal clinical events. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynocology, 136, 912–919.Google Scholar
  3. Bayley, N. (1969). Bay ley Scales of Infant Development: Birth to Two Years. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  4. Brazelton, T. B. (1973). Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (Clinics in Developmental Medicine No. 50 ). Philadelphia: Lippincott.Google Scholar
  5. Butler, B. V., & Engel, R. (1969). Mental and motor scores at 8 months in relation to neonatal photic responses. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 77, 77–82.Google Scholar
  6. Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1978). Manual of the home observation for measurement of environment. Unpublished manuscript available from the authors. University of Arkansas, Little Rock.Google Scholar
  7. Callaway, E., Tueting, P., & Koslow, S. (1978). Event-related brain potentials and behavior. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  8. Corballis, M. (1983). Human laterality. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  9. Cox, C., Hack, M., & Metz, D. (1981). Brainstem-evoked response audiometry: Normative data from the preterm infant. Audiology, 20, 53–64.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cox, L., Hack, M., & Metz, D. (1984). Auditory brain stem response abnormalities in the very low birth weight infant: Incidence and risk factors. Ear and Hearing, 5, 47–51.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dennis, M., & Whitaker, H. (1977). Hemispheric equipotentiality and language acquisition. In S. Segalowitz & F. Gruber (Eds.), Language development and neurological theory (pp. 93–106 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  12. Dunn, L. (1965). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  13. Engel, R., & Fay, W. (1972). Visual evoked responses at birth, verbal scores at three years, and IQ at four years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 14, 283–289.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Engel, R., & Henderson, N. B. (1973). Visual evoked responses and IQ scores at school age. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 15, 136–145.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Ertl, J. P. (1971). Fourier analysis of evoked potentials and human intelligence. Nature, 230, 525–526.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Ertl, J. P. & Schafer, E. W. P. (1969). Brain response correlates of psychometric intelligence. Nature, 223, 421–422.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Goff, W. R. Human average evoked potentials: procedures for stimulating and recording. In R. F. Thompson & M. M. Patterson, (Eds.), Bioelectric recording techniques, Part B. New York: Academic Press, 101–156.Google Scholar
  18. Henderson, N. B., & Engel, R. (1974). Neonatal visual evoked potentials as predictors of psychoeducational testing at age seven. Developmental Psychology, 10, 269–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Jasper, H. H. (1958). The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation of Societies for Electroencephalography: Appendix to report of the committee on methods of clinical examination in electroencephalography. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371–375.Google Scholar
  20. Jensen, D. R., & Engel, R. (1971). Statistical procedures for relating dichotomous responses to maturation and EEG measurements. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 30, 437–443.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  22. Littman, B., & Parmelee, A. (1978). Medical correlates of infant development. Pediatrics, 61, 470–474.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. McCarthy, D. (1972). Manual for the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  24. Molfese, D. L. (1972). Cerebral asymmetry in infants, children and adults: Auditory evoked responses to speech and noise stimuli. Dissertation accepted by the Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University.Google Scholar
  25. Molfese, D. L. (1980). The phoneme and the engram: Electrophysiological evidence for the acoustic invariant in stop consonants. Brain and Language, 9, 372–376.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Molfese, D. L. (1983). Event related potentials and language processes. In A. W. K. Gaillard & W. Ritter (Eds.), Tutorials in ERP research: Endogenous components (pp. 345–368 ). The Hague: North Holland Publishing Co.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Molfese, D. L., & Betz, J. C. (1988). Electrophysiological indices of the early development of lateralization for language and cognition, and their implication for predicting later development. In D. L. Molfese & S. J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Brain lateralization in children: Developmental implications (pp. 171–190 ). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  28. Molfese, D. L., Freeman, R., & Palermo, D. (1975). The ontogeny of lateralization for speech and nonspeech stimuli. Brain and Language, 2, 356–368.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (1979a). Hemisphere and stimulus differences as reflected in the cortical responses of newborn infants to speech stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 75, 505–511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (1979b). Infant speech perception: Learned or innate? In H. Whitaker & H. Whitaker (Eds.), Advances in neurolinguistics (Vol. 4, pp. 225–240 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  31. Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (1980). Cortical responses of preterm infants to phonetic and nonphonetic speech stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 16, 574–581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (1985). Electrophysiological indices of auditory discrimination in newborn infants: The bases for predicting later language development? Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 197–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (1986). Psychophysical indices of early cognitive processes and their relationship to language. In J. E. Obrzut & G. W. Hynd (Eds.), Child neuropsychology, Volume 1: Theory and research (pp. 95–116 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  34. Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (1988). Right hemisphere responses from preschool children to temporal cues contained in speech and nonspeech materials: Electrophysiological correlates. Brain and Language, 33, 245–259.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Molfese, D. L., & Schmidt, A. (1983). An auditory evoked potential study of consonant perception in different vowel environments. Brain and Language, 18, 57–70.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Molfese, D. L., & Searock, K. (1986). The use of auditory evoked responses at one year of age to predict language skills at 3 years. Australian Journal of Communication Disorders, 14, 35–46.Google Scholar
  37. Molfese, D. L., & Segalowitz, S. J. (1988). Brain lateralization in children: Developmental implications. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  38. Molfese, V. J. (1989). Perinatal risk and infant development: Assessment and prediction. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  39. Molfese, V. J., & Holcomb, L. C. (1989). Predicting learning and other developmental disabilities: Assessment of reproductive and caretaker variables. In N. W. Paul (Ed.), Research in infant assessment (pp. 1–24 ). New York: March of Dimes Birth Defects Foundation.Google Scholar
  40. Murray, A. (1988). Newborn auditory brainstem evoked responses (ABR’s): Longitudinal correlates in the first year. Child Development, 59, 1542–1554.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Murray, A., Dolby, R., Nation, R., & Thomas, D. (1981). Effects of epidural anesthesia on newborns and their mothers. Child Development, 52, 71–82.Google Scholar
  42. Nelson, C. A., & Salapatek, P. (1986). Electrophysiological correlates of infant recognition memory. Child Development, 57, 1482–1497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Regan, D. (1989). Human Brain Electrophysiology. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elseviev.Google Scholar
  44. Rockstroh, B., Elbert, T., Birbaumer, N., & Lutzenberger, W. (1982). Slow brain potentials and behavior. Baltimore: Urban & Schwarzenberg.Google Scholar
  45. Ruchkin, D., Sutton, S., Munson, R., & Macar, F. (1981). P300 and feedback provided by the absence of the stimulus. Psychophysiology, 18, 271–282.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Segalowitz, S. (1983). Language functions and brain organization. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  47. Segalowitz, S., & Gruber, F. (1977). Language development and neurological theory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  48. Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale ( 4th ed. ). Chicago: The Riverside Publishing Co.Google Scholar
  49. Travis, L. E. (1931). Speech pathology. New York: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag New York, Inc. 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Dennis L. Molfese

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations