Skip to main content

Cognitive Monitoring in Mathematics

  • Chapter
Cognitive Strategy Research

Abstract

The role of cognitive monitoring has been a dominant theme in cognitive strategy research in recent years. Cognitive monitoring can be defined as any activity aimed at evaluating or regulating one’s own cognitions (Flavell, 1979). Examples of cognitive monitoring include planning, checking, self-testing, assessing one’s progress, and correcting one’s errors. Researchers have come to recognize that cognitive monitoring plays a vital role in virtually any cogni­tive endeavor and thus has important implications for instructional interven­tion. Accordingly, cognitive monitoring has received a substantial amount of attention in the reading, communication, and memory literatures (e.g., Baker, 1985; Baker & Brown, 1984a, 1984b; Beal, this volume, chap. 8; Brown, 1978; Flavell, 1979; Markman, 1981).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • August, D.L., Flavell, J.H., & Clift, R. (1984). Comparison of comprehension monitoring of skilled and less skilled readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 39–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. (1984a) Children’s effective use of multiple standards for evaluating their comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 588–597.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. (1984b). Spontaneous versus instructed use of multiple standards for evaluating comprehension: Effects of age, reading proficiency and type of standard. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 289–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. (1985). How do we know when we don’t understand? Standards for evaluating text comprehension. In D.L. Forrest-Pressley, D.L. MacKinnon, & T.G. Waller (Eds.), Metacognition, cognition, and human performance (pp. 155–205). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L., & Brown, A.L. (1984a). Cognitive monitoring in reading. In J. Flood (Ed.), Understanding reading comprehension (pp. 21–44 ). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L., & Brown, A.L. (1984b). Metacognitive skills and reading. In P.D. Pearson (Ed.), Handbook of research in reading (pp. 353–393). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, A., Fischbein, E., & Greer, B. (1984). Choice of operation in verbal arithmetic problems: The effects of number size, problem structure, and context. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 15, 129–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J., Hasselbring, T., Barron, B., Kulewicz, S., Littlefield, J., & Goin, L. (in press). Use of macrocontexts to facilitate mathematical thinking. In R. Charles & E. Silver (Eds.), Teaching and evaluating mathematical problem solving. Reston, Va: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Briars, D.J., & Larkin, J.H. (1984). An integrated model of skill in solving elementary word problems. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 245–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.L. (1978). Knowing when, where, and how to remember: A problem of metacognition. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (pp. 77–165 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum & Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.L. Bransford, J.D., Ferrara, R.A., & Campione, J.G. (1983). Learning, remembering and understanding. In J.H. Flavell & E.M. Markman (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. III, Cognitive development (pp. 77–166). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charles, R.I., & Lester, E.K. (1984). An evaluation of a process-oriented mathematical problem solving instruction program in grades five and seven. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 15, 15–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, R.B. (1986). Conceptual and procedural knowledge in mathematics: A summary analysis. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowledge: The case of mathematics (pp. 265–300). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum & Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, R.B., & McKnight, C. (1980). The influence of semantic content on algorthmic behavior. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 3, 167–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeCorte, E., & Verschaffel, L. (1981). Children’s solution processes in elementary arithmetic problems. Journal of Education Psychology, 73, 765–779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeCorte, E., & Verschaffel, L. (1985). Beginning first graders’ initial representation of arithmetic word problems. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 4, 3–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott-Faust, D.J., & Pressley, M. (1986). How to teach comparison processing to increase children’s short-and long-term listening comprehension monitoring. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 27–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forrest-Pressley, D., & Waller, T.G. (1984). Cognition, metacognition and reading. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, R. (1981). Monitoring of passage inconsistency among poor comprehenders: A preliminary test of the “piecemeal processing” explanation. Journal of Educational Research, 74, 159–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garner, R. (1986). Metacognition and reading comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R., & Meek, E. (1986). The notion of principle: The case of counting. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowledge: The case of mathematics (pp. 29–57 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum & Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J.G., Riley, M.S., & Gelman, R. (1984). Conceptual competence and children’s counting. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 94–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, B. (1987). Nonconservation of multiplication and division involving decimals. Journal for Research on Mathematics Education, 18, 37–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hiebert, J. (Ed.). (1986). Conceptual and procedural knowledge: The case of mathematics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum & Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiebert, J., & Lefevre, P. (1986). Conceptual and procedural knowledge in mathematics: An introductory analysis. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowledge: The case of mathematics (pp. 1–27 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum & Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hope, J.A. (1986). Mental calculation: Anachronism or basic skill. In H.L. Schoen, & M.J. Zweng (Eds.), Estimation and mental computation (1986 yearbook of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, pp. 44–54). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kreutzer, M.A., Leonard, C., & Flavell, J.H. (1985). An interview study of children’s knowledge about memory. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 40, (1, Serial No. 159).

    Google Scholar 

  • Leon, J.A., & Pepe, H.J. (1983). Self-instructional training: Cognitive behavior modification for remediating arithmetic deficits. Exceptional Children, 50, 54–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lesh, R. (1981). Applied mathematical problem solving. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 12, 235–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesh, R., Landau, M., & Hamilton, E. (1983). Conceptual models and applied mathematical problem-solving. In R. Lesh & M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of mathematical concepts and processes (pp. 264–343 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lester, F.J. (1985). Methodological considerations in research on mathematical problem-solving. In E.A. Silver (Ed.), Teaching and learning mathematical problem solving (pp. 41–69 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum & Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lester, F.J., & Garafolo, J. (April 1982). Metacognitive aspects of elementary school students’ performance on arithmetic tasks. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maki, R.J., & Berry, S.L. (1984). Metacomprehension of text material. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 663–679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E.M. (1979). Realizing that you don’t understand: Elementary school children’s awareness of inconsistencies. Child Development, 50, 643–655.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E.M. (1981). Comprehension monitoring. In W.P. Dickson (Ed.), Children’s oral communication skills (pp. 61–84 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E.M., & Gorin, L. (1981). Children’s ability to adjust their standards for evaluating comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 320–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • May, L.J., Frye, S.M., & Jacobs, D.C. (1981). HBJ mathenatics (teacher’s ed., grade 3). New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E. (1982). Memory for algebra story problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 199–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E. (1985). Implications of cognitive psychology of instruction in mathematical problem solving. In E.A. Silver (Ed.), Teaching and learning mathematical problem solving: Multiple research perspectives (pp. 123–138 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum & Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merrill Publishing Company. (1987). Merrill mathematics (teacher’s ed., grade 3). Columbus, OH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Millet; G. (1987). The influence of self-instruction on the comprehension monitoring performance of average and above average readers. Journal of Reading Behavior, 19, 303–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, M., & Paris, S.G. (1978). Children’s metacognitive knowledge about reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 680–690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nesher, P.A. (1986). Are mathematical understanding and algorithmic performance related. For the Learning of Mathematics, 6, 2–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesher, P.A., & Katriel, T.A. (1977). A semantic analysis of addition and subtraction word problems in arithmetic. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 8, 251–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papert, S. (1980). Mindstorms: Children, computers, and powerful ideas. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S.G., Wixson, K.K., & Palincsar, A.M. (1986). Instructional approaches to reading comprehension. In E. Rothkopf (Ed.), Review of research in education (Vol 13, pp. 91–128 ). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Post, T.R. (1981). Fractions: Results and implications from national assessment. Arithmetic Teacher, 28, 26–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M. (1986). The relevance of the good strategy user model to teaching of mathematics. Educational Psychologist, 21, 139–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., Goodchild, E, Fleet, J., Zajchowski, R., & Evans, E.D. (in press). The challenges of classroom strategy instruction. The Elementary School Journal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L.B. (1982). Syntax and semantics in learning to subtract. In T.P. Carpenter; J.M. Moser, & T.A. Romberg (Eds.), Addition and subtraction: A cognitive perspective (pp. 136–154). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum & Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L.B. (1983). A developmental theory of number understanding. In H.P. Ginsburg (Ed.). The development of mathematical thinking (pp. 110–152 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L.B., & Omanson, S.F. (1987). Learning to understand arithmetic. In P. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 41–96 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum & Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reys, R.E. (1984). Mental computation and estimation: Past, present, and future. Elementary School Journal, 84, 547–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reys, R.E., Rybolt, J.F., Bestgen, B.J., & Wyatt, J.W (1982). Processes used by good computational estimators. Journal for Research on Mathematics Education, 13, 183–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riley, M.S., Greeno, J.G., & Heller; J.I. (1983). Development of children’s problem-solving ability in arithmetic. In H.P. Ginsburg (Ed.), The development of mathematical thinking (pp. 153–196). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A.H. (1982). Some thoughts on problem solving research and mathematics education. In F.J. Lester & J. Garafolo (Eds.), Mathematical problem solving: Issues in research (pp. 27–37 ). Philadelphia: The Franklin Institute Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A.H. (1983). Beyond the purely cognitive: Belief systems, social cognitions, and metacognitions as driving forces in intellectual performance. Cognitive Science, 7, 329–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silver, E.A. (1986). Using conceptual and procedural knowledge: A focus on relationships. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowledge: The case of mathematics (pp. 181–198 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum & Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slife, B.D., Weiss, J., & Bell, T. (1985). Separability of metacognition and cognition: Problem solving in learning disabled and regular students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 437–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, J., Fuson, K., Ham, M., & Sook Kim, M. (1986). An analysis of addition and subtraction word problems in American and Soviet elementary mathematics textbooks. Cognition and Instruction, 3, 153–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trafton, P.R. (1986). Teaching computational estimation: Establishing an estimation mind set. In H.L. Schoen & M.J. Zweng (Eds.), Estimation and mental computation (1986 yearbook of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, pp. 16–30 ). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Haneghan, J.P. (1986). Children’s detection of errors in word problems: Evidence for comprehension monitoring in math (Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland, Baltimore County 1986). Dissertation Abstracts International, 47B: 2650.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Lehn, K. (1983). On the representation of procedures in repair theory. In H.P. Ginsburg (Ed.), The development of mathematical thinking (pp. 201–253). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer-Verlag New York Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Van Haneghan, J.P., Baker, L. (1989). Cognitive Monitoring in Mathematics. In: McCormick, C.B., Miller, G.E., Pressley, M. (eds) Cognitive Strategy Research. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-8838-8_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-8838-8_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-8840-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4613-8838-8

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics