Advertisement

Interpersonal Abilities of Infants as Generators for Transmission of Language and Culture

  • Colwyn Trevarthen
Part of the Ettore Majorana International Science Series book series (SYLI)

Abstract

There have been great advances in our knowledge of communicative abilities of infants in the past decade.

A two-month-old may transmit emotional messages by facial expression, vocalization and gesture and engage in an affectional exchange with the mother by sight and sound alone. Microanalysis shows the interactions to be regulated by cognitive and motivational processes specific for interpersonal engagement. Disturbance of the maternal response leads to strong expressions of distress.

Communication develops rapidly as the infant expresses more autonomy of motives in interactions governed by humour and teasing and increasing interest in manipulation of objects. Maternal behavior changes accordingly, expanding the range of emotional color while taking up the infant’s exploratory and performatory interests.

Towards the end of the first year, game playing, which mothers ritualize in action songs and chants, is changed by the baby showing active interest in the mother’s directives and referential actions. Coventional uses of artifacts are acquired simultaneously with awareness of messages in speech.

We conclude that innate interpersonal abilities regulate and drive forward the growth of cooperative awareness and that first steps in language acquisition rest on mutual confidence. Further developments in learning retain a foundation in interpersonal skills by which interests, intentions and feelings are transmitted in infancy.

Keywords

Facial Expression Young Infant Game Playing Pitch Contour Expressive Behavior 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aldis, O. 1975, Play Fighting, New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  2. Alegria, J. and Noirot, E. 1978, “Neonate orientation behavior towards the human voice”, International Journal of Behavior Development, 1: 291–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Amsterdam, B. 1972, “mirror self-image reactions before age two”. Developmental Psychology, 5: 297–305.Google Scholar
  4. Bower, T. G. R. 1974, Development in Infancy, Freeman, San Francisco.Google Scholar
  5. Condon, W. S., and Sander, L. W. 1974, “Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: interactional participation and language acquisition”. Science, 183: 99–101.ADSCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Darwin, C. 1872, The Expression of Emotions in Animals and Man. Methuen, London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fraiberg, S. 1980, Clinical Studies in Infant Mental Health: The First Year of Life. Tavistock, London.Google Scholar
  8. Halliday, M. A. K. 1975, Learning How to Mean: Explorations in the Development of Language. Arnold, London.Google Scholar
  9. Hubley, P. and Trevarthen, C. 1979, “Sharing a task in infancy”, in: Social Interaction During Infancy, New Directions for Child Development, I. Uzgiris, ed., 4: 57–80.Google Scholar
  10. Hunt, J. McV. 1965, “Intrinsic motivation and its role in psychological development”, in: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, D. Levine, ed., pp. 189–282. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln.Google Scholar
  11. Klaus, M., and Kenneil, J. 1976, Maternal Infant Bonding. Mosby, St Louis.Google Scholar
  12. Maratos, O. 1973, The Origins and Development of Imitation in the First Six Months of Life. Ph.D. thesis, University of Geneva.Google Scholar
  13. Marton, P., Minde, K. and Ogilvie, J. 1981. “Mother-infant interactions and the preterm nursery: A sequential analysis”, in: Preterm Birth and Psychological Development, S. Friedman and M. Sigman, eds., Academic Press, New York. pp. 179–205.Google Scholar
  14. Marwick, H., Calam, R., and Trevarthen, C. 1982. Significant dimensions of personality in mothers of two year olds. (In preparation).Google Scholar
  15. Marwick, H. and Trevarthen, C. 1982, Adaptation of vocal expression in mother’s speech to infants. (In preparation).Google Scholar
  16. Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. H. 1977, “Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates”. Science, 198: 75–78.ADSCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Murray, L. 1980. The Sensitivities and Expressive Capacities of Young Infants in Communication with their Mothers. Ph.D. thesis, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
  18. Oster, H. 1978. “Facial expression and affect development”, in: The Development of Affect, M. Lewis and L. A. Rosenblum, eds., Plenum Press, New York. pp. 43–75.Google Scholar
  19. Papousek, H. 1967, “Experimental studies of appetitional behavior in human newborns and infants” in: Early Behavior Comparative and Developmental Approaches, H. W. Stevenson, E. H. Hess and H. R. Rheingold, eds., John Wiley and Sons, New York, pp. 249–277.Google Scholar
  20. Papousek, H. 1969, “Individual differences in learned responses in human infants”, in: Brain and Early Behavior, R. J. Robinson, ed., Academic Press, London and New York, pp. 251–263.Google Scholar
  21. Parmelee, A. H. 1981, “Auditory function and neurological maturation in preterm infants”, in: Preterm Birth and Psychological Development, S. Freidman and M. Sigman, eds, Academic Press, New York, pp. 127–150.Google Scholar
  22. Piaget, J. 1953, The Origins of Intelligence in the Child, Routledge and Kegan Paul, London.Google Scholar
  23. Piaget, J. 1970, “Piaget’s Theory”, in: Carmichael’s Manual of Child Psychology, P. H. Müssen, ed., John Wiley and Sons, New York, pp. 703–732.Google Scholar
  24. Spence, M. J. and DeCasper, A. J., 1982, “Human fetuses perceive maternal speech” Paper delivered at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Austin, Texas, March 1982.Google Scholar
  25. Sylvester-Bradley, B. 1980, A Study of Young Infants as Social Beings. Ph.D. thesis, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
  26. Sylvester-Bradley, B. 1981. “Negativity in early infant-adult exchanges and its developmental significance”, in: Communication in Development, P. Robinson, ed., Academic Press, London, pp. 1–37.Google Scholar
  27. Sylvester-Bradley, B. and Trevarthen, C. 1978, “Baby talk as an adaptation to the infant’s communication”, in: The Development of Communication, N. Waterson and C. Snow, ed., John Wiley and Sons, New York. pp. 75–92.Google Scholar
  28. Tinbergen, N. 1952, “‘Derived activities’, their causation, biological significance, origin and emancipation during evolution”. Quarterly Review of Biology, 27: 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Trevarthen, C. 1974a, “Conversations with a two-month-old”. New Scientist, 2 May, 230–235.Google Scholar
  30. Trevarthen, C. 1974b, “The psychobiology of speech development”, in: Language and Brain: Developmental Aspects Neurosciences Research Program Bulletin, E. H. Lenneberg, ed., Neurosciences Research Program, Boston. 12: 570–585.Google Scholar
  31. Trevarthen, C. 1979a, “Neuroembryology and the development of perception”, in: Human Growth: A Comprehensive Treatise, Vol. III. F. Falkner and J. M. Tanner, eds, Plenum, New York.Google Scholar
  32. Trevarthen, C. 1979b, “Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity”, in: Before Speech: The Beginnings of Human Communication, M. Bullowa, ed., Cambridge University Press, London.Google Scholar
  33. Trevarthen, C. 1980a, “Neurological development and the growth of psychological functions”, in: Developmental Psychology and Society, J. Sants, ed., Macmillans, London.Google Scholar
  34. Trevarthen, C. 1980b “The foundations of intersubjectivity: development of interpersonal and cooperative understanding in infants”, in: The Social Foundations of Language and Thought: Essays in Honor of J. S. Bruner. D. Olson, ed., W. W. Norton, New York.Google Scholar
  35. Trevarthen, C. 1982a, “Basic patterns of psychenetic change in infancy”, in: Dips in Learning. T. Bever, ed., N. J. Erlbaum, Hillsdale. (In press).Google Scholar
  36. Trevarthen, C. 1982b “Primary motives for cooperative understanding”, in: Social Cognition: Studies in the Development of Understanding. G. Butterworth and P. Light, eds., Harvester Press, London, pp. 77–109.Google Scholar
  37. Trevarthen, C., Murray, L., and Hubley, P. 1981, “Psychology of infants”, in: Scientific Foundations of Clinical Paediatrics. J. Davis and J. Dobbing, eds., William Heinemann Medical Books, London, (second edition).Google Scholar
  38. Tronick, E., Als, H., and Brazelton, T. B. 1979, Early development of neonatal and infant behavior, in: Human Growth: A Comprehensive Treatise, vol.III, F. Falkner and J.M. Tanner, eds., Plenum, New York.Google Scholar
  39. Watson, J. S. 1977, “Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness”, in: Origins of the Infant’s Social Responsiveness. E. B. Thoman, ed., Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1983

Authors and Affiliations

  • Colwyn Trevarthen
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghUK

Personalised recommendations