Skip to main content

The Development of Attribution Processes

  • Chapter
The Development of Social Cognition

Abstract

Analyses of naive causal explanations have a long and venerable history in both social and developmental psychology (Heider, 1944; Piaget, 1930). Heider (1944) postulated that any “change in the environment gains its meaning from the source to which it is attributed” (p. 372). This “inferred meaning” was for Heider the wellspring for all social behavior. Piaget was similarly impressed with the pivotal role of causal explantion in the development of children’s thinking. Indeed it was Piaget’s view that “the child’s basic orientation towards causal explantion intrudes throughout the length and breadth of his cognitive life” (Flavell, 1963, p. 284).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Abelson, R. P. (1976). Script processing in attitude formation and decision-making. In J. S. Carroll & J. W. Payne (Eds.), Cognition and social behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abelson, R. P. (1981). Psychological status of the script concept. American Psychologist, 36, 715–729

    Google Scholar 

  • Ausubel, D. P., & Schiff, H. M. (1954), The effect of incidental and experimentally induced experience in the learning of relevant and irrelevant causal relationships by children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 84, 109–123

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ball, W. A. (1973). The perception of causality in the infant. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banks, W. C. (1976). White preference in blacks: A paradigm in search of a phenomenon. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 1176–1186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bassili, J. N. (1976). Temporal and spatial contingencies in the perception of social events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 680–685.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berndt, T. J. (1977). The effect of reciprocity norms on moral judgment and causal attribution. Child Development, 48, 1322–1330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berzonsky, M. (1971). The role of familiarity in children’s explanations of physical causality. Child Development, 42, 705–715.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumenfeld, P., Bossert, S., Hamilton, U., Wesslo, C., & Mecce, J. (1979). Teacher talk and student thought: Socialization to the student role. Paper presented at the Conference on Teacher and Student Perceptions of Success and Failure, Pittsburgh, PA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borton, R. W. (1979). The perception of causality in infants. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, San Francisco.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bovee, G. R. (1985). The attributional and memorial consequences of processing covariation information. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. (1973). Judgments and explanations on criteria for the presence of cognitive structures. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 172–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. (1977). Response criteria in concept development. Child Development, 48, 360–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1966). On the conservation of liquids. In J. S. Bruner, R. Oliver, & P. Greenfield (Eds.), Studies in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, M., & Gelman, R. (1979). Preschool children’s assumptions about cause and effect: Temporal ordering. Child Development, 50, 89–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butzin, V. (1979). Children’s moral judgments of ulterior motives. Paper presented at the Biennial Convention of the Society for Research in Child Development, San Francisco.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carni, E., & French, L. (1984). The acquisition of before and after reconsidered: What develops? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 394–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1978). Intellectual development from birth to adulthood: A neo-Piagetian interpretation. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, E. A., Gelfand, D. M., & Hartmann, D. P. (1981). Causal reasoning as a function of behavioral consequences. Child Development, 52, 514–522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo, P., & Dix, T. (1983). Beyond the information processed: Socialization and the development of attribution processes. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo, P., Goie, J., Grumet, J., & Farnill, D. (1973). A re-examination of the effects of intent and consequence on children’s moral judgments. Child Development, 44, 154–161.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo, P., Grumet, J., & Brehm, S. (1974). The effects of choice and source of constraint on children’s attributions of preference. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 352–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crocker, J. (1981). Judgment of covariation by social perceivers. Psychological Bulletin, 9, 272–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalenberg, C. J., Bierman, K. L., & Furman, W. (1984). A reexamination of developmental changes in causal attributions. Developmental Psychology, 20, 575–583.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Andrade, R. G. (1981). The cultural part of cognition. Cognitive Science, 5, 179–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • DiVitto, B., & McArthur, L. Z. (1978). Developmental differences in the use of distinctiveness, consensus, and consistency information for making causal attributions. Developmental Psychology, 14, 474–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dix, T., & Herzberger, S. (1983). The role of logic and salience in the development of causal attribution. Child Development, 54, 960–967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58, (Whole No. 270).

    Google Scholar 

  • Farnill, D. (1974). The effects of social judgment set on children’s use of intent information. Journal of Personality, 42, 276–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (1981). On the self-perception explanation of the overjustification effect: The role of salience of initial attitude. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 417–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fineham, F. D. (1983). Developmental dimensions of attribution theory. In J. Jaspars, F. Fineham, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Attribution theory and research: Developmental and social dimensions. London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goody, J. (1977). The domestication of the savage mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grumet, J. (1975). Effects of adults’ and peer sanctions on children’s attributions of preference. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Duke University, Durham, NC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, J. H., Town, J. P., & Yarkin, K. L. (1981). How fundamental is “the fundamental attribution error”? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 346–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958) The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., & Bryant, S. L. (1982). Consensus information and the fundamental attribution error: The role of development and in-group versus out-group knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 889–900.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., & Parsons, J. E. (1983). Social cognition and the social life of the child: Stages as subcultures. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., Ruble, D. N., & Hartup, W. W. (1983). Social cognition and social development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1960). A treatise of human nature. Oxford: Clarendon (Originally published, 1740 ).

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. Kanouse, H. Kelley, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kant, I. (1961). Kritik der reinen vernunft. In W. Kaufmann (Ed.), Philosophic classics:Vol. 2. Bacon to Kant. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall (Original work published 1781 ).

    Google Scholar 

  • Karniol, R., & Ross, M. (1976). The development of causal attributions in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 455–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karniol, R., & Ross, M. (1979). Children’s use of a causal attribution schema and the influence of manipulative intentions. Child Development, 50, 463–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M. (1981). From “laychild” to “layman.” Developmental causal attribution. In S. Brehm, S. Kassin, & F. Gibbons (Eds.), Developmental social psychology: Theory and research. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M., & Baron, R. M. (1985). Basic determinants of attribution and social perception. In J. Harvey & G. Weary (Eds.), Attribution: Basic issues and applications (Vol. 1 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M., & Gibbons, F. X. (1981). Children’s use of the discounting principle in their perceptions of exertion. Child Development, 52, 71–744.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M., & Lepper, M. R. (1984). Oversufficient and insufficient justification effects: Cognitive and behavioral development. In J. Nicholls (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M., & Lowe, C. A. (1979). On the development of the augmentation principle: A perceptual approach. Child Development, 50, 728–734.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M., Lowe, C. A., & Gibbons, F. X. (1983). Children’s use of the discounting principle: A perceptual approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 718–728.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaye, H. (1967). Infant sucking behavior and its modification. In L. P. Lipsitt & C. C. Spiker (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, (Vol. 3 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1971). Causal schemata and the attribution process. Morristown, NJ: General Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessen, W. (1979). The American child and other cultural inventions. American Psychologist, 34, 815–820.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, G. A. (1947). Conditioning as a function of the time between conditioned and unconditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, 1–15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of gestaltpsychology. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1963). The development of children’s orientation toward a moral order: 1. Sequence in the development of moral thought. Vita Humana, 6, 11–33.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Alon, S., & Lewis, T. (1972). Retrospective misattribution and task enjoyment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 493–501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Schwartz, J. M., Maides, S., & Hamel, I. Z. (1978). Covariation, discounting, and augmentation: Towards a clarification of attribution principles. Journal of Personality, 46, 176–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., & Phelps, H. (1976). The development of children’s comprehension of causal direction. Child Development, 47, 248–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kun, A. (1977). Development of the magnitude-covariation and compensation schemata in ability and effort attributions of performance. Child Development, 48, 862–873.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kun, A. (1978). Evidence for preschoolers’ understanding of causal direction in extended causal sequences. Child Development, 49, 218–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (1979). Cross-cultural psychology’s challenges to our ideas of children and development. American Psychologist, 34, 827–832.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leahy, R. L. (1979). Development of conceptions of prosocial behavior: Information affecting rewards given for altruism and kindness. Developmental Psychology, 15, 34–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the “overjustiflcation” hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., Sagotsky, G., Dafoe, J., & Greene, D. (1982). Consequences of superfluous social constraints: Effects of nominal contingencies on children’s subsequent intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 51–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1982). The perception of causality in infants, Perception, 11 173–186.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lesser, H. (1977). The growth of perceived causality in children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 130, 145–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levi-Strauss, C. (1966). The savage mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipsitt, L. P., & Kaye, H. (1964). Conditioned sucking in the human newborn. Psychonomic Science, 2, 29–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Livesley, W., & Bromley, D. (1973). Person perception in childhood and adolescence. London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • McArthur, L. A. (1972). The how and what of why: Some determinants and consequences of causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 171–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • McArthur, L. Z., & Post, D. L. S. (1977). Figural emphasis on person perception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 520–535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, M. (1962). Coming of age in Samoa. New York: Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehan, H. (1979). Learning lessons. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mendelson, R. & Shultz, T. R. (1976). Covariation and temporal contiguity as principles of causal inference in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 22, 408–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. London: Metheun (Original work published in 1946 ).

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, M. (1983). Decrements in intrinsic motivation among rewarded and observer subjects. Child Development, 54, 636–644.

    Google Scholar 

  • Much, N. C. & Schweder, R. A. (1978). Speaking of rules: The analysis of culture in the breach. In W. Damon (Ed.), New directions for child development, Vol. 2: Moral development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1981). Social cognition in a script framework. In J. H. Flavell & L. Ross (Eds.), Social cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. (1977). Telling more than we can know verbal on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Odom, R. D., Astor, E. C., & Cunningham, J. G. (1975). Effects of perceptual salience on the matrix task performance of four and six-year-old children. Child Development, 46, 757–762.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olum, V. (1956). Developmental differences in the perception of causality. American Journal of Psychology, 69, 417–428.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orvis, B. R., Cunningham, J. D., & Kelley, H. H. (1975). A closer examination of causal inference: The roles of consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 605–616.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peevers, B., & Secord, P. (1973). Developmental changes in attribution of descriptive concepts to persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 120–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1930). The child’s conception of physical causality. London: Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1963/1932) The moral judgment of the child New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1974). Understanding causality. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pool, D. L., Shweder, R., & Much, N. (1983). Culture as a cognitive system: Differentiated role understanding in children and other savages. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pryor, J.B., & Kriss, M. (1977). The cognitive dynamics of salience in the attribution process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35, 49–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pryor, J. B., Rholes, W. S., Ruble, D. M., & Kriss, M. (In Press). A developmental analysis of salience and discounting in social attribution. Representative Research in Social Psychology

    Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1970). Experimental child psychology. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renner, K. E. (1964). Delay of reinforcement: An historical overview. Psychological Bulletin, 61, 341–361.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. M. Black & W. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rholes, W. S. & Ruble, D. N. (1984). Children’s understanding of dispositional characteristics of others. Child Development, 55, 550–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, M. (1975). Salience of reward and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 245–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. M., Feldmaft, N. S., Higgins, E. T., & Karlovac, M. (1979). Locus of causality and the use of information in the development of causal attributions. Journal of Personality, 47, 595–614.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salili, F., Maehr, M., & Gilmore, G. (1976). Achievement and morality: A cross- cultural analysis of attribution and evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 327–337.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, T. (1981). The acquisition of culture. Ethos, 9, 4–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedlak, A. J., & Kurtz, S. T. (1981). A review of children’s use of causal inference principles. Child Development, 52, 759–784.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaklee, H. (1976). Development in inferences of ability and task difficulty. Child Development, 47, 1051–1057.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaklee, H., & Tucker, D. (1979). Cognitive bases of development in inferences of ability. Child Development, 50, pp. 904–907.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharp, K. C. (1982). Preschoolers’ understanding of temporal and causal relations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 427–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shultz, T. R. (1982). Causal reasoning in the social and nonsocial realms. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 14, 307–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shultz, T. R., & Butkowski, I. (1977). Young children’s use of the scheme for multiple sufficient causes in the attribution of real and hypothetical behavior. Child Development, 48, 464–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shultz, T. R., Butkowsky, I., Pearce, J. W., & Shanfield, H. (1975). Development of schemes for the attribution of multiple psychological causes. Developmental Psychology, 11, 502–510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shultz, T. R., & Mendelson, R. (1975). The use of covariation as a principle of causal analysis: Child Development, 46, 394–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shultz, T. R, & Ravinsky, F. B. (1977). Similarity as a principle of causal inference. Child Development, 48, 1552–1558.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1975). Defining the locus of developmental differences in children’s causal reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 20, 512–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R S., & Liebert, R. M. (1974). Effects of contiguity, regularity, and age on children’s causal inferences. Developmental Psychology, 10, 574–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M. C. (1975). Children’s use of the multiple sufficient cause schema in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 737–747.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M. C. (1975). Children’s use of the multiple sufficient cause schema in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 737–747.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., Crocker, J., Fiske, S. T., Sprinzen, M., & Winkler, J. D. (1979). The generalizability of saliene effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 357–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1975). Point of view and perceptions of causality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 439–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive culture: Researchers into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, art and custom. London: Murray

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, D., & Shultz, T. R (1980). Developmental distinctions between behavior and judgment in the operation of the discounting principle. Child Development, 51, 1307–1310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L. (1980). Lay analyses of causal forces on behavior. In J. H. Harvey (Ed.), Cognition, social behavior and the environment. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. & Kaplan, E. (1956). The developmental approach to cognition: Its relevance to the psychological interpretive of anthropological and ethnolinguistie data. American Anthropologist, 58, 866–880.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, P. (1984). A model of the layperson as pragmatist. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 333–348.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1985 Springer-Verlag New York Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kassin, S.M., Pryor, J.B. (1985). The Development of Attribution Processes. In: Pryor, J.B., Day, J.D. (eds) The Development of Social Cognition. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-5112-5_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-5112-5_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-9569-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-5112-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics