Skip to main content

Apprenticeship in Word Use: Social Convergence Processes in Learning Categorically Related Nouns

  • Chapter
The Development of Word Meaning

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Cognitive Development ((2116))

Abstract

Research and theory aimed at understanding the development of word meaning has generated a wealth of information about early semantic development. To date, most of this work has focused on the task of describing changes in the child’s internal representations of lexical items and, to a lesser degree, on what has been called theprocess of acquisition. An important point about this vast body of prior research is its dependence on the implicit supposition that theacquisition process is a process localized within children. This supposition has persisted largely undisturbed because of widespread failure to fully appreciate the social embeddedness of cognitive development, in general, and of word learning, in particular. This chapter offers a markedly different perspective on the problems of semantic development—one built on foundations laid by Vygotsky and Wittgenstein, though drawing materials from the Piagetian, social learning, and learning-theoretic traditions as well.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adams, A. K. ( 1984, March).Methods for detecting dynamic regularities in parent-child verbal interaction. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Southwestern Society for Research in Human Development, Denver, CO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, A. K. (1984).Language and thought reconsidered: The role of linguistic tools in human cognitive activities. Unpublished manuscript. University of Denver.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anglin, J. M. (1977).Word, object, and conceptual development. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J. L. (1962).How to do things with words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, M. D. (1978). Lexical development and overextension in child language.Journal of Child Language,5, 205–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, M. D. (1982). Distinguishing between prototypes: The early acquisition of the meaning of object names. In S. A. Kuczaj (Ed.),Language development, Vol. 1: Syntax and semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, S., Gutfreund, M., Satterly, D., & Wells, G. (1983). Characteristics of adult speech which predict children’s language development.Journal of Child Language,70, 65–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1982). Functionalist approaches to grammar. In E. Wanner & L. R. Gleitman (Eds.),Language acquisition: The state of the art. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blewitt, P. (1982). Word meaning acquisition in young children: A review of theory and research.Advances in Child Development and Behavior,77, 139–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blewitt, P. (1983). “Dog” vs. “Collie”: Vocabulary in speech to young children.Developmental Psychology,79, 602–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blewitt, P. ( 1983, April).What determines order of acquisition of object categories? Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Detroit, MI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloor, D. (1983).Wittgenstein: A social theory of knowledge. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (1982). Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development. In E. Wanner & L. R. Gleitman (Eds.),Language acquisition: The state of the art. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L., Bransford, J. D., Ferrara, R. A., & Campione, J. C. (1983). Learning, remembering, and understanding. In P. Mussen (Ed.),Carmichael’s manual of child psychology: Vol. 3. Cognitive Development (pp. 77–166 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1958). How shall the thing be called?Psychological Review,65, 14–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1976). Reference: In memorial tribute to Eric Lenneberg.Cognition,4, 125–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1978). A new paradigm of reference. In G. A. Miller & E. Lenneberg (Eds.),Psychology and biology of language and thought: Essays in honor of Eric Lenneberg. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1975). From communication to language—A psychological perspective.Cognition,3, 255–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, D. (1979).Social coordination and children’s learning of property words. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, D. (1981). On the current and potential scope of generative theories of cognitive development. In K. W. Fischer (Ed.),Cognitive development. New Directions for Child Development, No. 12. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, D. (1983). Seeking relations between cognitive and social-interactive transitions. In K. W. Fischer (Ed.),Levels and transitions in children’s development. New Directions for Child Development, No. 21. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, D. ( 1984, March).Attentional dynamics and pattern completion processes as organizers of parent-child interaction. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Southwestern Society for Research in Human Development, Denver, CO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, D. ( 1984, March).Attentional dynamics and pattern completion processes as organizers of parent-child interaction. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Southwestern Society for Research in Human Development, Denver, CO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, D., & Zare, S. ( 1980, September).Determinants of word-choice in mothers’ speech to three-year-olds. Paper presented at the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Montreal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Callanan, M. ( 1983, April).Parental input and young children’s acquisition of hierarchically organized concepts. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Detroit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cross, T. G. (1977). Mothers’ speech adjustments: The contribution of selected child listener variables. In C. E. Snow & C. Ferguson (Eds.),Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danford, J. W. (1978).Wittgenstein and political philosophy: A reexamination of the foundations of social science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1975). Why the law of effect will not go away.Journal for the theory of social behavior,5, 169–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S. (1983).Joint picture-book reading as memory training for toddlers. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Detroit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K., & Suppes, P. (1974). Foundations of stimulus sampling theory. In D. H. Krantz et al. (Eds.),Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology. Volume I: Learning, memory, and thinking. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., & Mazzie, C. ( 1983, April).Pitch-marking of new and old information in mothers’ speech to infants. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Detroit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns.Developmental Psychology,20, 104–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills.Psychological Review,87, 477–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., & Bullock, D. (1984). Cognitive development in school-age children: Conclusions and new directions. In W. A. Collins (Ed.),Development during middle childhood. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goody, J. (1977).The domestication of the savage mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J., & Kuczaj, S. A. (1982). Towards a theory of substantive word-meaning acquisition. In S. A. Kuczaj (Ed.),Language development. Volume 1: Syntax and semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S. (1982).Studies of mind and brain. Boston: D. Reidel Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guillaume, P. (1971).Imitation in children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original French edition 1926 ).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, H., & Wexler, K. (1975). A mathematical theory of learning transformational grammar.Journal of Mathematical Psychology,12, 321–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hintikka, J. (1973).Logic, language-games and information. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horton, M. S., & Markman, E. M. (1980). Developmental differences in the acquisition of basic and superordinate categories.Child Development,51, 708–719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufmann, G. (1980).Imagery, language and cognition. Bergen: Universitetforlaget.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaye, K. (1982).The mental and social life of babies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaye, K., & Charney, R. (1980). How mothers maintain “dialogue” with two-year-olds. In D. R. Olson (Ed.),The social foundations of language and thought. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition. (1983). Culture and cognitive development. In R H. Mussen (Ed.),Manual of child psychology: Vol. I. History, theories, and methods (pp. 295–356 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1972).Hedges: A study in meaning criteria and the logic of fuzzy concepts. Pages from the Eighth Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistics Society. Chicago: University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lock, A. (1980).The guided reinvention of language. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucariello, J. ( 1983, April).Is the basic level always basic? Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Detroit.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (1984). Commentary: Where do categories come from? In C. Sophian (Ed.),Origins of cognitive skills. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M. (1983). Representation. In P. Mussen (Ed.),Carmichael’s manual of child psychology: Vol. 3. Cognitive development (pp. 420–494 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M. (1981). Two different principles of conceptual organization. In M. E. Lamb & A. L. Brown (Eds.),Advances in developmental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M. (1984). The acquisition and hierarchical organization of categories by children. In C. Sophian (Ed.),Origins of cognitive skills. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M., & Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children’s sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic vs. thematic relations.Cognitive Psychology,16, 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mervis, C. B. (1983). Acquisition of a lexicon.Contemporary Educational Psychology,8, 210–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mervis, C. B. (1984). Early lexical development: The contributions of mother and child. In C. Sophian (Ed.),Origins of cognitive skill. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mervis, C. B., & Canada, K. (1983). On the existence of competence errors in early comprehension: A reply to Fremgen & Fay and Chapman & Thompson.Journal of Child Language,10, 431–440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mervis, C. B., & Mervis, C. A. (1982). Leopards are kitty-cats: Object labeling by mothers for their thirteen-month-olds.Child Development,53, 267–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millikan, R. G. (1984).Language, thought, and other biological categories: New foundations for realism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moerk, E. L. (1976). Processes of language teaching and training in the interactions of mother-child dyad.Child Development,47, 1064–1078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moerk, E. L. (1983). A behavioral analysis of controversial topics in first language acquisition: Reinforcements, corrections, modeling, input frequencies, and the three-term contingency pattern.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research,12, 129–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1979). Explorations in the development of a functional semantic system. In W. A. Collins (Ed.),Children’s language and communication. The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, Vol 12. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1983). The derivation of concepts and categories from event representations. In E. K. Scholnick (Ed.),New trends in conceptual representation: Challenges to Piaget’s theory? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K., & Gruendel, J. (1981). Generalized event representations: Basic building blocks of cognitive development. In M. Lamb & A. Brown (Eds.),Advances in developmental psychology, Vol.1. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. E., & Nelson, K. (1978). Cognitive pendulums and their linguistic realization. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.),Children’s language, Vol.1. New York: Halstead Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ninio, A. (1980). Picture-book reading in mother-infant dyads belonging to two sub-groups in Israel.Child Development,57, 587–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ninio, A. (1983). Joint book reading as a multiple vocabulary acquisition device.Developmental Psychology,19, 445–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ninio, A., & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling.Journal of Child Language,5, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1955).The language and thought of the child. New York: Meridian Books. (Original work published 1923 )

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas.Journal ofExperimental Psychology,77, 353–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1970). Retention of abstract ideas.Journal of Experimental Psychology,83, 304–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratner, N., & Bruner, J. (1978). Games, social exchange and the acquisition of language.Journal of Child Language,5, 391–401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, L. A. (1981). Category development in early language.Journal of Child Language,8, 225–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.),Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblance: Studies in the internal structure of categories.Cognitive Psychology,8, 573–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories.Cognitive Psychology,8, 382–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., & Wertsch, J. V. (1984).Children’s learning in the “Zone of Proximal Development.” New Directions for Child Development, No. 23. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: The emergence of displaced reference in parent-child discourse. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.),Children’s language, Vol.4. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. B., Gearhart, M., & Guberman, S. R. (1984). The social organization of early number development. In B. Rogoff & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.),Children’s learning in the “Zone of Proximal Development.” New Directions for Child Development, No.23. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlesinger, I. M. (1982).Steps to language: Toward a theory of native language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1969).Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies.Journal of Child Language,4, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1983). Saying it again: The role of expanded and deferred imitations in language acquisition. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.),Children’s language, Vol.4. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., & Ferguson, C. A. (1977).Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Storm, C. (1980). The semantic structure of animal terms: A developmental study.International Journal of Behavioral Development,3, 381–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1983).Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1962).Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published in 1934 )

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978).Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published in 1934 )

    Google Scholar 

  • Wales, R., Colman, M., & Patterson, P. (1983). How a thing is called—A study of mothers’ and children’s naming.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,36, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1979). From social interaction to higher psychological processes: A clarification and application of Vygotsky’s theory.Human Development,22, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V., McNamee, G. D., McLane, J. B., & Budwig, N. A. (1980). The adult-child dyad as a problem solving system.Child Development,51, 1215–1221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, T. G. (1982). Naming practices, typicality, and underextension in child language.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,33, 324–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953).Philosophical investigations. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D. J. (1980).Teaching the young child: Some relationships between social interaction, language, and thought. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J. (1979). Concepts and classification. In G. Whitehurst & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.),The functions of language and cognition. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1986 Springer-Verlag New York Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Adams, A.K., Bullock, D. (1986). Apprenticeship in Word Use: Social Convergence Processes in Learning Categorically Related Nouns. In: Kuczaj, S.A., Barrett, M.D. (eds) The Development of Word Meaning. Springer Series in Cognitive Development. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-4844-6_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-4844-6_7

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-9326-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-4844-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics