How Children Learn the Reference of Concrete Nouns: A Critique of Current Hypotheses

  • William E. Merriman
Part of the Springer Series in Cognitive Development book series (SSCOG)

Abstract

This chapter will trace the historical development of several recent hypotheses about the acquisition of concrete noun reference. This history will begin with the many hypotheses proposed in the seminal papers of Clark (1973a) and Nelson (1974), since much attention over the last 10 years has focused on these hypotheses. Clark and Nelson have responded to developments subsequent to the publication of their hypotheses by revising, extending, and, in some cases, abandoning them as well as by contributing new hypotheses. Four avenues will be taken in critiquing the hypotheses of Clark and Nelson: (a) the hypotheses will be assessed for how well they account for the data they were designed to explain; (b) alternative explanations of these data will be sought; (c) the logical consistency of the hypotheses will be examined; and (d) research in which implications of the hypotheses have been confirmed or disconfirmed will be analyzed. The chapter will dwell on the inadequacies of the hypotheses of Clark and Nelson with the goal of developing a more accurate and more complete account of how children learn the reference of concrete nouns.

Keywords

Rubber Posit Bark Sorting Dinate 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adams, A. K., & Bullock, D. (1983). Anomaly and context effects in maternal labeling of category examplars. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Detroit.Google Scholar
  2. Andersen, E. S. (1975). Cups and glasses: Learning that boundaries are vague. Journal of Child Language, 2, 79–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anglin, J. (1977). Word, object, and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  4. Barrett, M. D. (1978). Lexical development and overextension in child language. Journal of Child Language, 5, 205–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bartlett, E. J. (1976). Sizing things up: The acquisition of the meaning of dimensional adjectives. Journal of Child Language, 5, 205–219.Google Scholar
  6. Benedict, H. (1978). Language comprehension in 9–15-month-old children. In R. N. Campbell & R T. Smith (Eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of language: Language development and mother-child interaction. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  7. Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: Comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language, 6, 183–200.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bierwisch, M. (1970). Semantics. In J. Lyons (Ed.), New horizons in linguistics. Harmondsworth, England: Pengu in Books.Google Scholar
  9. Blewitt, P. (1982). Word meaning acquisition in young children: A review of theory and research. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 17 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  10. Blewitt, P. (1983a). What determines order of acquisition of object categories? Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research on Child Development, Detroit.Google Scholar
  11. Blewitt, P. (1983b). Dog versus collie: Vocabulary in speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 19, 601–609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time—the use of single word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
  13. Bowerman, M. (1978a). The acquisition of word meaning: An investigation into some current conflicts. In N. Waterson & C. E. Snow (Eds.), The development of communication. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  14. Bowerman, M. (1978b). Semantic and syntactic development: A review of what, when, and how in language acquisition. In R. L. Schiefelbusch (Ed.), Bases for language intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  15. Bretherton, I., Bates, E., McNew, S., Shore, C., Williamson, C., & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Comprehension and production of symbols in infancy: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 17, 728–736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Brewer, W. E., & Stone, J. B. (1975). Acquisition of spatial antonym pairs. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 19, 299–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Brown, R. W. (1956). Language and categories. Appendix to J. S. Bruner, J. J. Goodnow, & G. A. Austin. A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  18. Brown, R. W. (1958). How shall a thing be called? Psychological Review, 65, 14–21.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Callanan, M. A. (1982). Parental input and young children’s acquisition of hierarchically organized concepts. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
  20. Callanan, M. A., & Markman, E. M. (1982). Principles of organization in young children’s natural language hierarchies. Child Development, 53, 1093–1101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Carey, S. (1982). Semantic development—state of the art. In L. Gleitman & E. Wanner (Eds.), Language development—state of the art. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  22. Chambers, J. C., & Tavuchis, N. (1976). Kids and kin: Children’s understanding of American kin terms. Journal of Child Language, 3, 62–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Chomsky, C. (1969). Acquisition of syntax in children from 5 to 10. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  24. Clark, E. V. (1971). On the acquisition of “before” and “after.” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 266–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Clark, E. V. (1973a). What’s in a word? On the child’s acquisition of semantics in his first language. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  26. Clark, E. V. (1973b). Non-linguistic strategies and the acquisition of word meanings. Cognition, 2, 161–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Clark, E. V. (1975). Knowledge, context, and strategy in the acquisition of meaning. In D. P. Dato (Ed.), Georgetown University round table on language and linguistics. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Clark, E. V. (1983). Meanings and concepts. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s Manual of Child Psychology: Vol. 3. Cognitive Development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  29. Clark, H., & Clark, E. V. (1977). Psychology and language: An introduction to psycholinguistics. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
  30. Cramer, P. (1974). Idiodynamic sets as determinants of children’s false recognition errors. Developmental Psychology, 10, 86–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Currie-Jedermann, J. (1981). The role of junction in conceptual development. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society of Research in Child Development, Boston.Google Scholar
  32. Daehler, M. W., Lonardo, R., & Bukatko, D. (1979). Matching and equivalence judgments in very young children. Child Development, 50, 170–179.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Dale, P. S. (1976). Language development: Structure and function. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
  34. Danziger, K. (1957). The child’s understanding of kinship terms: A study in the development of relational concepts. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 91, 213–232.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. deVos, L. E, & Caramazzo, A. (1977). The role of form and Junction in the development of natural language concepts. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans.Google Scholar
  36. Donaldson, M., & Balfour, G. (1968). Less is more: A study of language comprehension in children. British Journal of Psychology, 59, 461–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Donaldson, M., & Wales, R. J. (1970). On the acquisition of some relational terms. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  38. Eilers, R. E., Oiler, K. K., & Ellington, J. (1974). The acquisition of word meaning for dimensional adjectives: The long and the short of it. Journal of Child Language, 1, 195–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Fremgen, A., & Fay, D. (1980). Overextensions in production and comprehension: A methodological clarification. Journal of Child Language, 7, 205–212.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Gentner, D. (1978). On relational meaning: The acquisition of verb meaning. Child Development, 49, 988–998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Glucksberg, S., Hay, A., & Danks, J. H. (1976). Words in uttterance contexts: Young children do not confuse the meanings of same and different. Child Development, 47, 737–741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Goldin-Meadow, S., Seligman, M. E. P., & Gelman, R. (1976). Language in the two-year- old. Cognition, 4, 189–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Haviland, S. E., & Clark, E. V. (1974). ‘This man’s father is my father’s son’: A study of the acquisition of English kin terms. Journal of Child Language, 1, 23–48.Google Scholar
  44. Horton, M. S., & Markman, E. M. (1980). Developmental differences in the acquisition of basic and superordinate categories. Child Development, 51, 708–719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Howard, D. V., & Howard, J. H. (1977). A multidimensional scaling analysis of the development of animal names. Developmental Psychology, 13, 108–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehension. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology: the Loyola symposium. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  47. Katz, N., Baker, E., & MacNamara, J. (1974). What’s in a name? A study of how children learn common and proper nouns. Child Development, 45, 469–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Kay, D. A., & Anglin, J. (1982). Overextension and underextension in the child’s expressive and receptive speech. Journal of Child Language, 9, 83–98.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Kuczaj, S. A. (1982). Children’s overextensions in comprehension and production: Support for a prototype theory of object word meaning acquisition. First Language, 3, 93–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Labov, W. (1973). The boundaries of words and their meanings. In C-J. N. Bailey & R. Shuy (Eds.), New ways of analysing variation in English. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
  51. Labov, W., & Labov, T. (1974). The grammar of “cat” and “mama”. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, New York.Google Scholar
  52. Lewis, M. M. (1951). Infant speech: A study of the beginnings of language. New York: Humanities Press.Google Scholar
  53. Markman, E. M., Horton, M. S., & McLanahan, A. G. (1980). Classes and collections: Principles of organization in the learning of hierarchical relations. Cognition, 8, 227–242.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. McCarthy, D. A. (1954). Language development in children. In L. Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of child psychology (2nd ed.).Google Scholar
  55. Mervis, C. B., & Canada, K. (1981). Child-basic categories and early lexical development. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston.Google Scholar
  56. Mervis, C. B., & Crisafi, M. A. (1982). Order of acquisition of subordinate-, basic-, and superordinate-level categories. Child Development, 53, 258–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Mervis, C. B., & Mervis, C. A. (1982). Leopards are kitty-cats: Object labeling by mothers for their thirteen-month-olds. Child Development, 53, 267–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Mervis, C. B., & Pani, J. R. (1980). Acquisition of basic object categories. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 496–522.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Mervis, C. B., & Rosch, E. (1981). Categorization of natural objects. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology (Vol. 32 ). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews Inc.Google Scholar
  60. Miller, G. A., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1976). Language and perception. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  61. Nelson, K. (1973b). Some evidence for the cognitive primacy of categorization and its functional basis. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 19, 21–39.Google Scholar
  62. Nelson, K. (1974). Concept, word, and sentence: Interrelations in acquisition and development. Psychological Review, 81, 267–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Nelson, K. (1976). Some attributes of adjectives used by young children. Cognition, 4, 13–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Nelson, K. (1977). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift revisited: A review of research and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 93–116.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Nelson, K. (1978). Semantic development and the development of semantic memory. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s language (Vol. 1 ). New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
  66. Nelson, K. (1979a). Explanations in the development of a functional semantic system. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Children’s language and communication—the Minnesota symposium on child psychology: Vol. 12. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  67. Nelson, K. (1979b). Features, contrasts, and the FCH: Some comments on Barrett’s lexical development hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 6, 139–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Nelson, K., Rescorla, L., Gruendel, J., & Benedict, H. (1978). Early lexicans: What do they mean? Child Development, 49, 960–968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Nelson, K. E., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1975). Semantic retrieval in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 11, 807–813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Perlmutter, M., & Ricks, M. (1979). Recall in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 423–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Piaget, J. (1928). Judgment and reasoning in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Postal, P. M. (1966). Andre Martinet, ‘Elements of general linguistics’. Foundations of Language, 2, 151–186.Google Scholar
  73. Prawat, R. S., & Cancelli, A. A. (1977). Semantic retrieval in young children as a function of type of meaning. Developmental Psychology, 13, 354–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Prawat, R. S., & Wildfong, S. (1980). The influence of functional context on children’s labeling responses. Child Development, 51, 1057–1060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Reich, P. (1976). The early acquisition of word meaning. Journal of Child Language, 3, 117–123.Google Scholar
  76. Rescorla, L. A. (1980). Overextension in early language development. Journal of Child Language, 7, 321–335.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Rescorla, L. A. (1981). Category development in early language. Journal of Child Language, 8, 225–238.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Richards, M. M. (1979). Sorting out what’s in a word from what’s not: Evaluating Clark’s semantic features acquisition theory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 1–47.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Rosch, E. H. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  80. Rosch, E. H., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Rosch, E. H., Simpson, C., & Miller, R. S. (1976a). Structural bases of typicality effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 491–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Rosch, E. H., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W., Johnson, D., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976b). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Rosch, E. H. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. H. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  84. Ross, G. S. (1980). Categorization in 1- to 2-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 16, 391–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Slobin, D. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In C. A. Ferguson & D. Slobin (Eds.), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
  86. Snow, C. E. (1977). Mother’s speech research: From input to interaction. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  87. Suchman, R. G., & Trabasso, T. (1966). Stimulus preference and cue function in young children’s concept attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 188–198.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Thomson, J., & Chapman, R. W. (1977). Who is ‘Daddy’ revisited: The status of two-year- olds’ overextended words in use and comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 4; 359–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Tomikawa, S. A., & Dodd, D. H. (1980). Early word meanings: Perceptually or functionally based? Child Development, 57, 1103–1109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Vosniadou, S., & Ortony, A. (1983). The emergence of the literal-metaphorical-anomalous distinction in young children. Child Development, 54, 154–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. (E. Haufmann, & G. Vakar, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  92. Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  93. Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1964). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  94. Whitehurst, G. J., Kedesdy, J., & White, T. G. (1982). A functional analysis of meaning. In S. A. Kuczaj (Ed.), Language development (Vol. 1 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag New York Inc. 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • William E. Merriman

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations