Skip to main content

Assessment of Attention, Simultaneous-Successive Coding, and Planning

  • Chapter
Interactive Assessment

Part of the book series: Disorders of Human Learning, Behavior, and Communication ((HUMAN LEARNING))

Abstract

There has been a significant shift away from the definition of abilities toward an interest in understanding cognitive processes (Glaser, 1972; Messick, 1973). In response to this shift from ability to process, some psychologists have turned to information processing (e.g., Hunt, 1980; Simon, 1981) because the broad framework that information processing provides can accommodate disparate approaches to intelligence such as computer-based and neuropsychological models. Understanding how information is processed requires some attempt to comprehend how it occurs in the brain. This intention to understand the proverbial “black box” is shared by intelligence theorists such as Eysenck (1981) and Jensen (1981), especially evidenced through their attempts to discover a biological foundation for intelligence.

Present trends suggest a receding of the term intelligence concurrent with a shift of research emphasis from the goal of refining the definitions of abilities in order to further their measurement. (Estes, 1976, p. 304)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Arlin, P. K. (1977). Piagetian operations in problem finding. Developmental Psychology, 13, 297–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armitage, S. F. (1946). An analysis of certain psychological traits used for the evaluation of brain damage. Psychological Monographs, 60 (Whole No. 277).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashman, A. (1978). The relationship between planning and simultaneous and successive synthesis. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C, & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), Advances in the psychology of learning and motivation research and theory (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1981). From the percept to the cognitive structure. In J. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance (Vol. 9, pp. 1–26). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, A. (1974). Luria’s neuropsychological investigation. Copenhagen, Denmark: Munksgaard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowalt, C. A., & McCallum, R. S. (1984). Simultaneous-successive processing across the life-span: A cross-sectional examination of stability and proficiency. Experimental Aging Research, 10(4), 225–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P. (1972). Patterns of cognitive ability in nonretarded and retarded children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 77, 6–12.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P. (1973). Cultural deprivation and cognitive competence. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 6). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P. (1980). Planning: Theoretical considerations and empirical evidence. Psychological Research (W. Germany), 41, 141–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P. (1984a). Intelligence and information integration. In J. Kirby (Ed.), Cognitive strategies and educational performance. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P. (1984b). Aspects of planning. In J. Kirby (Ed.), Cognitive strategies and educational performance. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P. (1984c). Simultaneous and successive processes and K-ABC. Journal of Special Education, 18, 229–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P. (1985a). Remedial training for the amelioration of cognitive deficits in children. In A. F. Ashman & R. S. Laura (Eds.), The Education and training of the mentally retarded (pp. 215–244). London: Croom Helm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P. (1985b). Aspects of digit span performance: Naming time and order memory. American Journal of mental Deficiency, 89, 627–634.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P., & Bower, A. C. (1971). Originating responses of mentally retarded and normal children to work signals. British Journal of Psychology, 62, 89–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P., & Dash, U. N. (1983). Hierarchical factor solution of coding and planning processes: Any new insights. Intelligence, 7, 27–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P., & Heemsbergen, D. B. (1983). Planning as a factor in the assessment of cognitive processes. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 1, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P., Kirby, J., & Jarman, R. F. (1975). Simultaneous and successive synthesis: An alternative model for cognitive abilities. Psychological Bulletin, 82(1), 87–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P., Kirby, J. E., & Jarman, R. (1979). Simultaneous and successive cognitive processes. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P., & Molloy, G. N. (1975). Varieties of simultaneous and successive processing in children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 213–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P., & Naglieri, J. A. (in preparation). Cognitive assessment system.

    Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P., & Varnhagen, C. K. (1986). Neuropsychological functioning and cognitive processing. Child Neuropsychology, 1, 117–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. (1976). Intelligence and cognitive psychology. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), The Nature of intelligence (pp. 295–306). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. (1981). Intelligence and learning. In M. Friedman, J. P. Das, & N. O’Connor (Eds.), Intelligence and learning, New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1982). A model for intelligence. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Furby, L. (1974). Attentional habituation and mental retardation. In Human Development, 17, 118–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garofalo, J. (1986). Simultaneous synthesis, regulation, and arithmetical performance. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 4, 229–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N. (1982). Disorders of attention: a frontier in neuropsychology. In D. E. Broadbent & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), The Neuropsychology of cognitive function. Philosophical transaction of the Royal Society of London (pp. 173–185).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E., & Rader, N. (1979). Attention. In G. A. Hale & Lewis M. (Eds.), Attention and cognitive development (pp. 1–22). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, R. (1972). The new aptitudes. Educational Researcher, 1, 5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, F. K., & Kendall, B. S. (1960). Memory-for-designs test: Revised general manual. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 11, 147–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heemsbergen, D. S. (1980). Planning as a cognitive process. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hess, W. R. (1967). Casulty, consciousness and cerebral organization. Science, 158, 1279–1283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, D., & Randhawa, B. S. (1983). Cognitive processes and achievement. The Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 29(3), 206–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E. B. (1980). Intelligence as an information processing concept. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 449–474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jarman, R. F., & Das, J. P. (1977). Simultaneous and successive synthesis and intelligence. Intelligence, 1, 151–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarman, R. F. (1980). Cognitive processes and syntactic structure: Analyses of syntagmatic and paradigmatic associations. Psychological Research, 41, 153–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1981). Reaction time and intelligence. In M. P. Friedman, J. P. Das, & N. O’Connor (Eds.), Intelligence and learning (pp. 39–50). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Karrer, R. (Ed.). (1976). Developmental psychophysiology of mental retardation. Springfield, II: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behaviour. In J. A. Jeffreys (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior: The Hixon symposium. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K. (1974). Spatial-temporal information processing in disabled readers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lezak, M. D. (1976). Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1966a). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1966b). Human brain and psychological processes. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1969). The origin and cerebral organization of man’s conscious action. A lecture given to the XIX International Congress of Psychology, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1970). The functional organization of the brain. Scientific American, 222, 66–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain. Middlesex, England: Pengu in Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man (2nd ed). New York: Basic Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R., & Artem’eva, E. Y. (1970). Two approaches to an evaluation of the reliability of psychological investigations. Soviet Psychology, 8, 271–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R., & Vinogradova, O. S. (1959). An objective investigation of the dynamics of semantic system. British Journal of Psychology, 50, 89–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCallum R. S., & Merritt, F. M. (1983). Simultaneous and successive processing among college students. Journal of Psychoeducational Research, 1, 85–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Messick, S. (1973). Multivariate models of cognitive and personality: The need for both process and structure in psychological theory and measurement. In J. Royce (Ed.), Contributions of multivariate analysis to theoretical psychology. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E. & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of Behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Murti, T. R. V. (1980). The central philosophy of Buddhism. London: Unwin Paperbacks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naglieri, J. A. (1985). Matrix Analogies Test-Short Form. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naglieri, J. A., & Das, J. P. (1988). Planning-Arousal-Simultaneous-Successive (PASS): A model for assessment. Journal of School Psychology, 26, 35–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parasuraman, R. (1983). Vigilance, arousal and the brain. In A. Gale & J. Edwards (Eds). Physiological correlates of human behavior (pp. 35–55). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parasuraman, R., & Davies, D. R. (Eds.). (1984). Varieties of attention. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1928). Lectures on conditioned reflex (W. H. Gantt, Trans). New York: International Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. R., & Eccles, J. C. (1978). The self and its brain. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometrie exploration of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Boies, S. J. (1971). Components of attention. Psychological Review, 78, 391–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pribram, K. H., & Luria, A. R. (1973). The frontal lobes and the regulation of behavior. In R. H. Pribram & A. R. Luria (Eds.), Psychology and the frontal lobes. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pribram, K. H., & McGuinness, D. (1975). Arousal, activation and effort in the control of attention. Psychological Review, 82, 116–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Randhawa, B. S., & Hunt, D. (1979). Some further evidence on successive and simultaneous integration and individual differences. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 77(4), 340–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C. (1955). Coloured Progressive Matrices Sets A, AB, B. London: H. K. Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C. (1958). Standard Progressive Matrices Sets A, B, C, D and E. London: H. K. Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reitan, R. M. (1956). The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 19, 393–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryckman, D. B. (1981). Reading achievement, IQ, and simultaneous-successive processing among normal and learning disabled children. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 27, 74–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schofield, N. J., & Ashman, A. F. (1986). The relationship between digit span and cognitive processing across ability groups. Intelligence, 10, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T., & Evans, M. E. (1978). The involvement of the frontal lobes in cognitive estimation. Cortex, 14, 294–303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1981). Studying human intelligence by creating artificial intelligence. American Scientist, 69(3), 300–309.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spreen, O., & Gaddes, W. H. (1969). Developmental norms for 15 neuropsychological tests age 5 to 15. Cortex, 5, 171–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.). (1985). Human abilities. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1984). Neuropsychological studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological Bulletin, 95(1), 3–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society—the development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wachs, M. C, & Harris, M. (1986). Simultaneous and successive processing in university students: Contribution to academic performance. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 4, 103–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Das, J.P., Naglieri, J. (1992). Assessment of Attention, Simultaneous-Successive Coding, and Planning. In: Haywood, H.C., Tzuriel, D. (eds) Interactive Assessment. Disorders of Human Learning, Behavior, and Communication. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-4392-2_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-4392-2_8

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-8753-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-4392-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics