Skip to main content

Implications of the Resource Allocation Model for Mood Disorders

  • Chapter
The Behavioral High-Risk Paradigm in Psychopathology

Part of the book series: Series in Psychopathology ((PSYCHOPATHOLOGY))

  • 148 Accesses

Abstract

Although classified as a mood disorder in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1987), depression is also known to have important cognitive features. Patients diagnosed with depression often report difficulties in concentration, attention, learning, and memory (Tariot & Weingartner, 1986), and numerous studies document the cognitive impairments that occur with this disorder (e.g., Glass, Uhlenhuth, Hartel, Matuzas, & Fischman, 1981; Silberman, Weingartner, & Post, 1983; Weingartner, Cohen, Murphy, Martello, & Gerdt, 1981; for reviews see McAllister, 1981; W.R. Miller, 1975; Mineka & Sutton, 1992). Tasks requiring sustained effort appear to be particularly likely to produce disruptions during cognitive processing in depression (Cohen, Weingartner, Smallberg, Pickar, & Murphy, 1982; Roy-Byrne, Weingartner, Bierer, Thompson, & Post, 1986). For example, depressed patients have been found to perform relatively poorly on tasks that require effortful or controlled processing (e.g., recalling a long list of items) but have less difficulty on tasks that can be accomplished automatically (e.g., recalling the location of furniture in a room that they just occupied; see Tariot & Weingartner, 1986). In short, considerable evidence indicates that clinical levels of depression are accompanied by cognitive deficits, particularly when depressives are presented with effort-demanding tasks.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Akiskal, H.S. (1983). Dysthymic disorder: Psychopathology of proposed chronic depressive subtypes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 11–20.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaney, P.H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D.T., & Fiske, D.W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cannon, W.B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: A critical examination and an alternative theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, L.J., Chapman, J.P., & Raulin, M.L. (1976). Scales for physical and social anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 374–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R.M., Weingartner, H., Smallberg, S.A., Pickar, D., & Murphy, D.L. (1982). Effort and cognition in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 593–597.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Depue, R.A., & Klein, D.N. (1988). Identification of unipolar and bipolar affective conditions in nonclinical and clinical populations by the General Behavior Inventory. In D.L. Dunner, E.S. Gershon, & J.E. Barrett (Eds.), Relatives at risk for mental disorders (pp. 179–202 ). New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Depue, R.A., Krauss, S., Spoont, M.R., & Arbisi, P. (1989). General Behavior Inventory identification of unipolar and bipolar affective conditions in a nonclinical university population. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 117–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donchin, E. (1981). Surprise!… Surprise? Psychophysiology, 18, 493–513.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donchin, E., Kramer, A.F., & Wickens, C. (1986). Applications of brain event-related potentials to problems in engineering psychology. In M.G.H. Coles, E. Donchin, & S.W. Porges (Eds.), Psychophysiology: Systems, processes, and applications (pp. 702–718 ). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, E.A. (1934). Emotion: An example of the need for re-orientation in psychology. Psychological Review, 41, 184–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H.C. (1985). On the importance of mood intensity and encoding demands in memory: Commentary on Hasher, Rose, Zacks, Sanft, and Doren. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 392–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H.C., & Ashbrook, P.W. (1988). Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states on memory. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition and social behavior (pp. 25–43 ). Toronto: C.J. Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H.C., Thomas, R.L., McFarland, A.D., & Lane, J.W. (1985). Emotional mood states and retrieval in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 363–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H.C., Thomas, R.L., & Rodriguez, I.A. (1984). Emotional mood states and memory: Elaborative encoding, semantic processing, and cognitive effort. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 470–482.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, G.L. (1931). Mental activity and the muscular processes. Psychological Review, 38, 428–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, A., & Poison, M.C. (1981). Hemispheres as independent resource systems: Limited-capacity processing and cerebral specialization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 1031–1058.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gjerde, P.F. (1983). Attentional capacity dysfunction and arousal in schizophrenia. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 57–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glass, R.M., Uhlenhuth, E.H., Hartel, F.W., Matuzas, W., & Fischman, M.W. (1981). Cognitive dysfunction and imipramine in outpatient depressives. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 1048–1051.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goplerud, E., & Depue, R.A. (1985). Behavioral response to naturally occurring stress in cyclothymia and dysthymia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 128–139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C., Davila, J., Brown, G., Ellicott, A., & Gitlin, M. (1992). Psychiatric history and stress: Predictors of severity of unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 45–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C., Mayol, A., deMayo, R., & Marks, T. (1986). Initial symptom levels and the life-event-depression relationship. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 114–122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., Rose, K.C., Zacks, R.T., Sanft, H., & Doren, B. (1985). Mood, recall, and selectivity effects in normal college students. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 104–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R.T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., Zacks, R.T., Rose, K.C., & Doren, B. (1985). On mood variation and memory: Reply to Isen (1985), Ellis (1985), and Mayer and Bower (1985). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 404–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P.T., & Hardin, T.S. (1990). Remembering with and without awareness in a depressed mood: Evidence of deficits in initiative. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 45–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P.T., & Rude, S.S. (1991). Degressive deficits in memory: Focusing attention improves subsequent recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 301–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hockey, G.R.J. (1970a). Changes in attention allocation in a multi-component task under loss of sleep. British Journal of Psychology, 61, 473–480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hockey, G.R.J. (1970b). Effect of loud noise on attentional selectivity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 28–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hockey, G.R.J., Coles, M.G.H., & Gaillard, A.W.K. (1986). Energetical issues in research on human information processing. In G.R.J. Hockey, A.W.K. Gaillard, & M.G.H. Coles (Eds.), Energetics and human information processing (pp. 3–21 ). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J.E., Houck, M.R., MacMillan, III, F.W., Simons, R.F., & Oatman, L.C. (1985). Event-related potentials elicited by automatic targets: A dual-task analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 50–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R.E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: Review and a conceptual model. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 156–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A.M. (1985). Asymmetry of happiness and sadness in effects on memory in normal college students: Comment on Hasher, Rose, Zacks, Sanft, and Doren. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 388–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isreal, J.B., Chesney, G.L., Wickens, C.D., & Donchin, E. (1980). P300 and tracking difficulty: Evidence for multiple resources in dual-task performance. Psychophysiology, 17, 259–273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, M.B., Lavori, P., Endicott, J., Coryell, W., & Klerman, G.L. (1983). “Double depression”: Two-year follow up. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 689–694.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, D.N. ( 1990, November). Dysthymia: A critical examination of the construct. Invited paper presented at the fifth annual meeting of the Society for Research on Psychopathology, Boulder, CO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, D.N., Dickstein, S., Taylor, E.T., & Harding, K. (1989). Identifying chronic affective disorders in outpatients: Validation of the General Behavior Inventory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 106–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, M., Feinberg, T.L., Crouse-Novak, M., Paulauskas, S., Pollock, M., & Finkelstein, R. (1984). Depressive disorders in childhood: II. A longitudinal study of the risk for a subsequent major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 643–649.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, A.F., Sirevaag, E., & Braune, R. (1987). A psychophysiological assessment of operator workload during simulated flight session. Human Factors, 29, 145–160.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, A.F., & Strayer, D.L. (1988). Assessing the development of automatic processing: An application of dual-task and event-related brain potential methodologies. Biological Psychology, 26, 231–267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, A.F., Wickens, C.D., & Donchin, E. (1983). An analysis of the processing demands of a complex perceptual-motor task. Human Factors, 25, 597–622.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, A.F., Wickens, C.D., & Donchin, E. (1985). Processing of stimulus properties: Evidence for dual-task integrality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 393–408.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krames, L., & MacDonald, M.R. (1985). Distraction and depressive cognitions. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 561–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leight, K.A., & Ellis, H.C. (1981). Emotional mood states: Strategies, and state-dependency in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 251–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A. (1990). Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 28, 455–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J.D., & Bower, G.H. (1985). Naturally occurring mood and learning: Comment on Hasher, Rose, Zacks, Sanft, and Doren. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 396–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAllister, T.W. (1981). Cognitive functioning in the affective disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 22, 572–586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A., Yee, C.M., & Anhalt, J.M. (1992). ERP incentive effects and potential risk for psychopathology. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, W.R. (1975). Psychological deficit in depression. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 238–260.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., & Sutton, S.K. (1992). Cognitive biases and the emotional disorders. Psychological Science, 3, 65–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1984). Resources—A theoretical soup stone? Psychological Review, 91, 216–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D., & Gopher, D. (1979). On the economy of the human-processing system. Psychological Review, 86, 214–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D., & Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 435–448.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1987). Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention. In H. Heuer & A.F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 361–394 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D.A., & Bobrow, D.G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nuechterlein, K.H., & Dawson, M.E. (1984). Information processing and attentional functioning in the developmental course of schizophrenic disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 10, 160–203.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roy-Byrne, P.P., Weingartner, H., Bierer, L.M., Thompson, K., & Post, R.M. (1986). Effortful and automatic cognitive processes in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 265–267.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A.F. (1983). Towards a model of stress and human performance. Acta Psychologica, 53, 61–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silberman, E.K., Weingartner, H., & Post, R.M. (1983). Thinking disorder in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40, 775–780.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sirevaag, E.J., Kramer, A.F., Coles, M.G.H., & Donchin, E. (1989). Resource reciprocity: An event-related brain potentials analysis. Acta Psychologica, 70, 77–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strayer, D.L., & Kramer, A.F. (1990). Attentional requirements of automatic and controlled processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 67–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tariot, P.N., & Weingartner, H. (1986). A psychobiologic analysis of cognitive failures. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 1183–1188.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weingartner, H., Cohen, R.M., Murphy, D.L., Martello, J., & Gerdt, C. (1981). Cognitive processes in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 42–47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, C.D. (1980). The structure of attentional resources. In R. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 239–258 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, C.D. (1984, April-May). The multiple resources model of human per-formance: Implications for display design. In AGARD/NATO Proceedings, Williamsburg, VA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, C.D. (1986). Gain and energetics in information processing. In G.R.J. Hockey, A.W.K. Gaillard, & M.G.H. Coles (Eds.), Energetics and human information processing (pp. 373–389 ). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, C.D., Kramer, A., Vanasse, L., & Donchin, E. (1983). The performance of concurrent tasks: A psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information processing resource. Science, 221, 1080–1082.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yee, C.M., Deldin, P.J., & Miller, G.A. (1992). Early stimulus processing in dysthymia and anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 230–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yee, C.M., & Miller, G.A. (1988). Emotional information processing: Modulation of fear in normals and dysthymics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 54–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yee, C.M., & Miller, G.A. (1994). A dual-task analysis of resource allocation in dysthymia and anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 625–636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yee, C.M., Miller, G.A., & Kramer, A.F. (1987). Resource demands of a recognition running-memory task. (Abstract) Psychophysiology, 24, 622.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1995 Springer-Verlag New York, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Yee, C.M. (1995). Implications of the Resource Allocation Model for Mood Disorders. In: Miller, G.A. (eds) The Behavioral High-Risk Paradigm in Psychopathology. Series in Psychopathology. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-4234-5_10

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-4234-5_10

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-8705-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-4234-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics