Advertisement

Epistemic Beliefs and Their Developmental Relationship to Post-Positivist Psychology

  • Marcia Salner
Conference paper
Part of the Recent Research in Psychology book series (PSYCHOLOGY)

Summary

This paper describes post-positivist epistemological assumptions and their development since the 1950s. As a vehicle for organizing the discussion, two recent articles in American Psychologist (Manicas & Secord, 1983, and Gergen, 1985) are reviewed and compared. Both articles have a similar purpose–to describe post-positivist psychology - but they differ in terms of how social reality is conceptualized. This dif - ference points to an open issue in theoretical psychology and to the role of epistemic beliefs in the world views that psychologists accept or reject. In order to emphasize the need for more attention to the psychological development of epistemic beliefs (and of psychological theorizing itself), the historical evolution of post-positivist assumptions in psychology is viewed through the lens of Perry’s (1970) theory of intellectual development. The structural similarities between individual intellectual development and the evolution of formal epistemology suggest that both individual and collective advances in psychological and philosophical reasoning take place against the same social background and result from socially given dilemmas and problems that individuals must confront.

Keywords

Scientific Realism Epistemic Belief Truth Claim Formal Operation Epistemological Development 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Arlin, P. K. (1986). Problem finding and young adult cognition. In R. A. Mines & K. S. Kitchener (Eds.), Adult Cognitive Development (pp. 22–23 ). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  2. Armon, C. (1984). Ideals of the good life and moral judgment: Ethical reasoning across the lifespan. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations (pp. 357–380 ). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  3. Basseches, M. (1986). Dialectical thinking and young adult cognitive development. In R. A. Mines & K. S. Kitchener (Eds.), Adult cognitive development (pp 33–56 ). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  4. Benack, S. (1984). Postformal epistemologies and the growth of empathy. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations (pp. 340–356 ). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  5. Berger, P. L., & Luckman, T. (1966). The social construction of reality: A treatise In the sociology of knowledge New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
  6. Bhaskar, R. (1975). A realist theory of science. Leeds, England: Leeds Books.Google Scholar
  7. Bhaskar, R. (1978). On the possibility of social scientific knowledge and the limits of behaviorism. Journal for the theory of social behavior, 8, 1–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bhaskar, R. (1979). The possibility of naturalism. Brighton, England: Harvester.Google Scholar
  9. Bhaskar, R. (1982). Emergence, explanation and emancipation. In P. F. Secord (Ed.),Explaining social behavior: Consciousness, behavior, and social structure(pp. 275–310 ). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  10. Coan, R. (1979). Psychologists: Personal and theoretical pathways. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
  11. Diamond, S. R., & Royce, J. R. (1980). Cognitive abilities as expressions of three “ways of knowing”. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 15, 31–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Eiduson, B. T. (1962). Scientists: Their psychological world. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  13. Gergen, K. (1985). The social constructionist movement in modern psychology. American Psychologist, 40, 266–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Harré, R. (1970). The principles of scientific thinking. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  15. Harré, R. (1972). Philosophies of science. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Harré, R., & Madden, E. H. (1975). Causal powers. Totawa, NJ: Littlefield Adams.Google Scholar
  17. Kitchener, K. S. (1983). Cognition, metacognition and epistemic cognition: A three-level model of cognitive processing. Human development, 26, 222–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kitchener, K. S. (1986). The reflective judgment model: Characteristics, evidence, and measurement. In R. A. Mines & K. S. Kitchener (Eds.), Adult cognitive development (pp. 76–91 ). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  19. Kitchener, K. S., & King, P. M. (1981). Reflective judgment: Concepts of justification and their relationship to age and education. Journal of Applied Development Psychology, 2, 89–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press (1st ed., 1962 ).Google Scholar
  21. Leplin, J. (Ed.). (1984). Scientific realism. Berkeley/Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  22. Mahoney, M. J. (1979). Psychology of a scientist: An evaluative view. Social Studies of Science, 9, 349–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Manicas, P. T., & Secord, P. F. (1983). Implications for psychology of the new philosophy of science. American Psychologist, 38, 399–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Maslow, A. (1966). The psychology of science: A reconnaissance. Chicago: Henry Regnery.Google Scholar
  25. Perry, W. (1970). Forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  26. Piaget, J. (1970). Genetic epistemology. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
  27. Powell, A., & Royce, J. R. (1978). Paths to being, life style and individuality. Psychological Reports, 42, 98–1005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development in young adults. In R. A. Mines & K. S. Kitchener (Eds.), Adult cognitive development (pp. 92–111 ). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  29. Roe, A. (1952). The making of a scientist. New York: Dodd, Mead.Google Scholar
  30. Royce, J. R. (1974). Cognition and knowledge: Psychological epistemology. In C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception, vol. 1: History and philosophical roots of perception (pp. 149–175 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  31. Royce, J. R. (1975). Epistemic styles, individuality and world view. In A. Debons & W. J. Cameron (Eds.), Perspectives in information science (pp. 259–295 ). Leyden, The Netherlands: International Publishing Co.Google Scholar
  32. Royce, J. R., & Rozeboom, W. W. (1972). The psychology of knowing. New York: Gordon and Breach.Google Scholar
  33. Royce, J. R., Coward, H., Egan, E., Kessel, F., & Mos, L. (1978). Psychological epistemology: A critical review of the empirical literature and the theoretical issues. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 97, 265–353.Google Scholar
  34. Sinnott, J. D. (1984). Postformal reasoning: The relativistic stage. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations (pp. 298–325 ). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag New York Inc. 1988

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marcia Salner
    • 1
  1. 1.Saybrook InstituteSan FranciscoUSA

Personalised recommendations