Psychological Conceptions of Mathematics and Emotion

  • Barbara A. McDonald

Abstract

Mathematics and emotion. Isn’t mathematics supposed to be nonemotional? Isn’t mathematics the essence of pure, objective, precise, untarnished thought? As educators, however, we know that many students have emotional reactions, often negative ones, to mathematics. Furthermore, it is my contention that every thought has an emotional component. The connection between cognitive and emotional processing (if they are separate processes at all) is fundamental. The participants in the Conference on Affective Issues in Mathematical Problem Solving were concerned with mathematics and emotion because they have recognized that learning is not all cognitive, that cognition and emotion are intertwined. We are attempting to understand the connection. The conference gave us an opportunity to discuss the cognitive-emotional interaction, define it for mathematics, understand the process, suggest methods for studying it, and find examples that support our views.

Keywords

Assure Caffeine Hull Fishing Defend 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anderson, K.J., & Revelle, W. (1982). Impulsivity, caffeine, and proofreading: A test of the Easterbrook hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 614–624.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Beck, A.T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  3. Benbow, C., & Stanley, J. (1980). Sex differences in mathematics ability: Fact or artifact? Science, 210, 1262–1264.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Betz, N.E., & Hackett, G. (1983). The relationship of mathematics self-efficacy expectations to selection of science-based college majors. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 23, 329–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Betz, N.E., & Hackett, G. (1986). Applications of self-efficacy theory to understanding career choice behavior. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 4, 279–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bower, G.H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Clifford, M.M. (1984). Thoughts on a theory of constructive failure. Educational Psychologist, 19, 108–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Clifford, M.M. (1988). Failure tolerance and academic risk-taking in ten- to twelve-year old students. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, 15–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Deci, E. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  10. Dweck, C.S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Epstein, S. (1973). The self-concept revisited. American Psychologist, 28, 404–416.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fennema, E., & Sherman, J. (1977). Sex-related differences in mathematics achievement, spatial visualization, and affective factors. American Educational Research Journal, 14, 51–71.Google Scholar
  13. Freud, S. (1975). The unconscious. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete works ofSigmund Freud (Vol. 14, pp. 159–215). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published in 1915.)Google Scholar
  14. Heckhausen, H., & Kuhl, J. (1985). From wishes to action: The dead ends and short cuts on the long way to action. In M. Frese & J. Sabini (Eds.), Goal directed behavior: The concept of action in psychology (pp. 133–159). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  15. Humphreys, M.S., & Revelle, W. (1984). Personality, motivation, and performance: A theory of the relationship between individual differences and information processing. Psychological Review, 91, 153–184.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Izard, C.E. (1984). Emotion-cognition relationships and human development. In C.E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R.B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, and behavior (pp. 17–37). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Jung, C.G. (1960). Instinct and the unconscious. In H. Read, M. Fordham, G. Adler (Eds.), & R.F.C. Hull (Trans.), The structure and dynamics of the psyche: Collected works of C.G. Jung (Vol. 8, pp. 129–138). New York: Pantheon Books. (Original work published in 1919.)Google Scholar
  18. Kagan, J. (1984). The idea of emotion in human development. In C.E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R.B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, and behavior (pp. 38–72). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. (1988). Motivation and cognitive abilities: An integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  20. Kuhl, J. (1986). Motivation and information processing: A new look at decision making, dynamic change, and action control. In S. Sorrentino & E.T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 404–434). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  21. Lazarus, R.S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psychologist, 39, 124–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Leon, M.R., & Revelle, W. (1985). The effects of anxiety on analogical reasoning: A test of three theoretical models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1302–1315.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Leventhal, H., & Tomarken, A.J. (1986). Emotion: Today’s problems. Annual Review of Psychology, 37, 565–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  25. Mandler, G. (1984). Mind and body: Psychology of emotion and stress. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  26. Marcel, A.J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology, 75, 238–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Marshall, S.P., & Smith, J.D. (1987). Sex differences in learning mathematics: A longitudinal study with item and error analyses. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79(4), 372–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Marshall, S.P., Pribe, C.A., & Smith, J.D. (1987). Schema knowledge structures for representing and understanding arithmetic story problems (Tech. Rep. Contract No. N00014–85-K-0661). Arlington, VA: Office of Naval Research.Google Scholar
  29. McDonald, B.A. (1987). The subtleties of cognitive and emotional expression. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  30. Parsons, J.E., Meece, J.L., Adler, T.E, & Kaczala, C.M. (1982). Sex differences in attributions and learned helplessness. Sex Roles, 8(4), 421–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Revelle, W. (1987). Personality and motivation: Sources of inefficiency in cognitive performance. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 436–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Sarason, I.G. (1975). Anxiety and self-preoccupation. In I.G. Sarason & C.D. Spielberger (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 2, pp. 27–44). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
  33. Shevrin, H., & Dickman, S. (1980). The psychological unconscious: A necessary assumption for all psychological theory? American Psychologist, 55, 677–688.Google Scholar
  34. Tomkins, S.S. (1979). Script theory: Differential magnification of affects. InH.E. Howe, Jr., & R.A. Dienstbier (Eds.), 1978Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 201–236). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  35. Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag New York Inc. 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • Barbara A. McDonald

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations