Skip to main content

What Is a Lie? Children’s Use of Intentions and Consequences in Lexical Definitions and Moral Evaluations of Lying

  • Chapter
Children’s Interpersonal Trust

Abstract

The temptation to tell a lie is likely to arise so often in children’s normal social interactions that, on the basis of frequency alone, it undoubtedly stands out from most of the other moral transgressions that children are called on to resist. Piaget (1932) argued that “lies present to the child’s mind a far graver and more pressing problem than do clumsiness or even such exceptional actions as stealing” (p. 135). In addition to sheer opportunity, Piaget felt that the lie’s salience as a moral temptation reflected the fact that “the tendency to tell lies is a natural and spontaneous tendency, so spontaneous and universal that we can take it as an essential part of the child’s egocentric thought” (p. 135). But as if this were not enough, the adult aiming to socialize children to avoid lying is likely to have an additional problem to contend with. Most everyday instructional or punitive interventions by parents, teachers, and other adult disciplinarians are guided by the assumption that a child’s understanding of words such as lies, truth, and honesty matches the meanings of these terms in the adult lexicon. Thus, discipline normally strives more to create the motivation to be honest than to explicate the underlying meanings of lying or telling the truth. Recent research, however, suggests that this popular assumption is flawed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Allison, A. (1986). Credibility of the child witness: Psychological/legal concerns. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 4(4), 33–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berliner, S., & Barbieri, M.K. (1984). Testimony of the child victim of sexual assault. Journal of Social Issues, 40, 125–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, L., & Nass, G. (1962). Social class differences in conscience development.Child Development, 33, 565–574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond, M.H. (1986). The psychology of the Chinese people. Oxford UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bok, S. (1978).Lying: Moral choices in public and private life. New York: Pantheon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, G.S. (1984). Children’s testimony in historical perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 40, 9–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, G.S. (1984). Child witness: Conclusions and future directions for research and legal practice. Journal of Social Issues, 40, 157–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, R., & Bell, R.A. (1984) Broadening the deception construct. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 70, 288–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C.N., & Wellman, H.M. (1980). Children’s developing understanding of mental verbs: Remember, know, & guess. Child Development, 51, 1095–1103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karniol, R. (1978). Children’s use of intention cues in evaluating behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 76–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kant, I. (1963). Lectures on ethics. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S.O. (1986). Developmental changes in the conception of lying in Korean children. Unpublished master’s thesis, Yonsei University, Seoul, S. Korea.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lickona, T. (1976). Research on Piaget’s theory of moral development. In T. Lickona (Ed.), Moral development and behavior (pp. 211-230). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindskold, S., & Han, G. (1986). Intent and the judgment of lies. Journal of Social Psychology, 126, 129-130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindskold, S., & Waters, P.S. (1983). Categories of acceptability of lies. Journal of Social Psychology, 120, 129–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maier, R.A., & Lavrakas, P.J. (1976). Lying behavior and the evaluation of lies. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 42, 575–581.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melton, G.B. (1981). Children’s competency to testify. Law and Human Behavior, 5, 73–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, A.H. (1974, May). Perception of manipulative behavior as a function of the manipulator, the person manipulated, and the goal of the manipulator. Paper presented at the meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C.C. (1979). Severity of consequences as a factor in moral judgement. In P. Taylor (Ed.), Piagetian theory and the helping professions (pp. 78–82). LosAngeles: U.S.C. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C.C. (1989). Telling the truth with deceptive intentions: Adults’ lexical and moral judgments. Unpublished manuscript, Murdoch University, Western Australia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C.C, & Kim, S.O. (1988). Lexical and moral judgements of deceit in Australia and South Korea. Paper presented at the 24th International Congress of Psychology, Sydney, Australia

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C.C, Peterson, J., & Seeto, D. (1983). Developmental changes in ideas about lying. Child Development, 54, 1529–1535.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C.C, & Peterson, M. (1990). Childre’s and adults lexical and moral judgments of lies told in courtroom versus domestic contexts. Unpublished manuscript, Murdoch University, Western Australia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge, & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pope, W.R., & Forsyth, D.R. (1986). Judgments of deceptive communications: A multidimensional analysis. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 24, 435–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pynoos, R. S., & Eth, S. (1984). The child as witness to homicide. Journal of Social Issues, 40(2), 87–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Song, M.J., Smetana, J., & Kim, S.Y. (1987). Korean children’s conceptions of moral and conventional transgressions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 577–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, C, & Stern, W. (1909). Monographien über die seelische Entwicklung des Kindes. Leipzig, Germany: Barth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, W. (1910). Abstracts of lectures on the psychology of testimony and on the study of individuality. American Journal of Psychology, 21, 273–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler v. United States, 159 US 523 (1895).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., Gruber, S., & Perner, J. (1984). Young children’s conception of lying: Lexical realism-moral subjectivism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1991 Springer-Verlag New York Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Peterson, C.C. (1991). What Is a Lie? Children’s Use of Intentions and Consequences in Lexical Definitions and Moral Evaluations of Lying. In: Rotenberg, K.J. (eds) Children’s Interpersonal Trust. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-3134-9_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-3134-9_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-7808-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-3134-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics