Skip to main content

The Constructive Function of Language in the Baby’s Development from Sensorimotor Adaptation to Humanity

  • Chapter
Sociogenesis Reexamined
  • 75 Accesses

Abstract

The French writer Vercors devotes his whole novel Les Animaux dénaturés to the question of defining criteria for humanity. Facing the creatures they named the “Tropis” (an easy way to avoid choosing between anthrôpos and pithêkos), discoverers meet with a moral, as well as an epistemological problem. If the Tropis are animals, nothing prevents the discoverers from having an instrumental, though decent relation to them. If they are humans, the matter is of course quite different: they are potential interlocutors.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Asch, S.E. (1952), Social psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barthes, R. (1964), Eléments de sémiologie. Communications, 4, 91–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. Volterra, V. (1979), The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, E.E. (1991), Symbolic action theory and cultural psychology. Berlin/New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonnet, C. (1980), L’enfant et le symbolique: l’accès aux premières structures sémiotiques. Paris: Vrin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borel, M.J. (1987), Discours descriptif et référence. Cahiers du centre de recherches sémiologiques (Neuchâtel), 53, 77–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1990), Politeness theory: exemplar and exemplary. In I. Rock (ed.), The legacy of Solomon Asch: essays in cognition and social psychology. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlabum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunswik, E. (1952), The conceptual framework of psychology. In O. Neurath, R. Carnap, C. Morris (eds.), Foundations of the unity of science, Vol.1. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 1–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassirer, E. (1929), Philosophie der symbolischen Formen, Band III. Berlin: Bruno Cassirer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D.L., Seyfarth, R.M. (1980), Vocal recognition in free-range Vervet monkeys. Animal behaviour, 28, 362–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Church, J. (1961), Language and the discovery of reality: a developmental psychology of cognition. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dallet, K. (1974), Transactional and probabilistic functionalism. In: E.C. Carterette, M.P. Friedman (eds.), Handbook of perception, Vol.1. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Frisch, K. (1950), Bees: their vision, chemical senses, and language. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallup, G.G. (1977), Self recognition in primates. American journal of psychology, 32, 329–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillièron, C. (1986), La validité en psychologie: première, deuxième ou troisième personne? Archives de psychologie, 54, 201–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillièron, C. (1987a), Du tiers inclus au tiers exclu: le rôle du langage dans la construction du sujet bien pensant. Revue européenne des sciences sociales, 25, 115–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillièron, C. (1987b), Is Piaget’ s “genetic epistemology” evolutionary? In W.R. Pinxten (ed.), Evolutionary epistemology. Dordrecht: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillièron, C. (1988), Les avatars du non. Cahiers du centre de recherches sémiologiques (Neuchâtel), 56, 55–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P.M. Smith, J.H. (1976), The structure of communication in early language development. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammond, K.R. (ed.)(1966), The psychology of Egon Brunswik. New York: Holt, Rinehart, Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1926), Ding und Medium. Symposion, 1, 109–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herbert, M.J. Harsh, C.M. (1944), Observational learning by cats. Journal of comparative psychology, 37, 81–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985), A constructivist approach to modelling linguistic and cognitive development. Archives de psychologie, 53, 113–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986), From meta-processes to conscious access: evidence from children’ s metalinguistic and repair data. Cognition, 23, 95–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1970), Gestalt perception as a source of scientific knowledge. InStudies in animal and human behaviour, Vol. II. London: Methuen. (Original 1959).

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945), Phénoménologie de la perception. Paris: Gallimard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mounoud, P., Vinter, A. (eds.) (1981), La reconnaissance de son image chez l’enfant et l’animal. Neuchâtel: Delachaux Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1936/1952), The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1937/1954), The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1946/1951), Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1947), Du rapport des sciences avec la philosophie. Synthese, 6, 130–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. Inhelder, B.(l941/1974), The child’ s construction of quantities: conservation and atomism. London: Routledge Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J, Inhelder, B.(1966/1969), The psychology of the child. London: Routledge Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piatelli-Palmarini, M. (ed.) (1979), Théories du langage, théories de l’apprentissage: Le débat entre Jean Piaget et Noam Chomsky. Paris: Seuil.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prieto, L.J. (1966), Messages et signaux. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prieto, L.J. (1975), Pertinence et pratique: essai de sémiologie. Paris: Editions de Minuit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1978), Meaning and the moral sciences. London: Routledge Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quadranti, P. (1989), Approche constructiviste de Vintersubjectivité comme préalable à la langue. Conférence présentée au “Internationales Kolloquium ’Sprache und Denken: Variation und Invarianz in Linguistik und Nachbardisziplinen’,” Lenzburg (Schweiz), 17–19 Mai.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rivière, A. (1990), Origen y desarrollo de la función simbòlica en el nino. In J. Palacios, A. Marchesi & C.Coll (eds.), Desarollo psicològico y educación, VoI. Madrid: Alianza.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rivière, A. & Coll, C. (1987), Individuation et interaction avec [sic] le sensorimoteur: notes sur la construction génétique du sujet et de l’ objet social. In M. Siguan (ed.), Comportement, cognition, conscience: la psychologie à la recherche de son objet. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. (1940), An inquiry into meaning and truth. London: Allen Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saussure, F. de (1916/1969), Cours de linguistique générale. Paris: Payot.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaerlaekens, A.M. (1973), The two-words sentence in child language development: a study based on evidence provided by Dutch-speaking triplets. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherrington, C. (1906), The integrative action of the nervous system. London: Scribner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitz, R.A. (1957), No and Yes: on the genesis of human communication. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Straus, E. (1935), Vom Sinn der Sinne. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thinès, G. (1977), Phenomenology and the science of behaviour. London: Allen Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thommen, E. (1991), La genèse de la perception de l’intentionnalité dans le mouvement apparent. Archives de psychologie, 59, 195–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E.C. Brunswik, E. (1935), The organism and the causal texture of the environment. Psychological review, 42, 43–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trân Duc Thao. (1973), Recherches sur l’origine du langage et de la conscience. Paris: Editions sociales.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vercors [J. Bruller] (1952), Les animaux dénaturés. Paris: Albin Michel.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, P.A., de Villiers, J.G. (1979), Early language. London: Fontana.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Weizsäcker, V. (1939), Der Gestaltkreis: Theorie der Einheit von Wahrnehmen und Bewegen. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodruff, G., Premack, D. (1979), Intentional communication in the chimpanzee: the development of deception.Cognition, 7, 33–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1994 Springer-Verlag New York, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gillièron, C. (1994). The Constructive Function of Language in the Baby’s Development from Sensorimotor Adaptation to Humanity. In: de Graaf, W., Maier, R. (eds) Sociogenesis Reexamined. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2654-3_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2654-3_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-7622-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-2654-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics