Advertisement

Bidirectional Cultural Transmission and Constructive Sociogenesis

  • Jaan Valsiner

Abstract

Contemporary psychology is remarkable for the discrepancy between the socially promoted beiief in the speedy advancement of the discipline, and the actually very slow progress in the basic ideas and understanding of the pertinent phenomena. The case of sociogenesis is exemplary in this respect: psychologists have been talking about the relevance of the social world for the formation of the psychological functions of persons for over a century, but still there is very little progress in building explicit theoretical models that could explain how the individual becomes a person via social relationships.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ananiev, B.G. (1930), Sociogenetic method in the study of human behavior. In A.B. Zalkind (ed.), Psikhonevrologicheskie nauki v SSSR. (pp. 29–33). Moscow-Leningrad: Gosudarstvennoie Meditsinskoe Izdatel’stvo.Google Scholar
  2. Bakhtin, M. (1981), The dialogic imagination. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
  3. Baldwin, J.M. (1892), Suggestion and will. International Congress of Experimental Psychology. Second Session (pp. 49–54). London: Williams & Norgate.Google Scholar
  4. Baldwin, J.M. (1895), Mental development in the child and the race.New York: MacMillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baldwin, J.M. (1902), Social and ethical interpretations in mental development. New York: MacMillan.Google Scholar
  6. Baldwin, J.M. (1906), Thought and things, or genetic logic. Vol. 1. Functional logic, or genetic theory of knowledge. London: Swan Sonnenschein.Google Scholar
  7. Baldwin, J.M. (1908), Thought and things, or genetic logic. Vol. 2. Experimental logic, or genetic theory of thought. London: Swan Sonnenschein.Google Scholar
  8. Baldwin, J.M. (1911), Thought and things, or genetic logic. Vol. 3. Interest and art.London: Allen.Google Scholar
  9. Baldwin, J.M. (1930), James Mark Baldwin. In C. Murchison (ed.), A history of psychology in autobiography. Vol. 1. (pp. 1–30). New York: Russell & Russell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bartlett, F.C. (1920a), Some experiments on the reproduction of folkstories. Folk-Lore 31, 30–47.Google Scholar
  11. Bartlett, F.C. (1920b), Psychology in relation to the popular story. Folk-Lore, 31, 264–293.Google Scholar
  12. Basov, M. (1931), Obshchie osnovy pedologii. Moscow-Leningrad: Gosudarst vennoe Izdatel’stvo.Google Scholar
  13. Basov, M. (1991), The organization of processes of behavior. In J. Valsiner & R. Van der Veer (eds.), Structuring of conduct in activity settings: The forgotten contributions of Mikhail Basov. Part 1. Soviet Psychology, 29, 5, 14–83.Google Scholar
  14. Bateson, G. (1972), Metalogue: What is an instinct? In G. Bateson, Steps to an ecology of mind. (pp. 38–58). New York: Ballantine Books.Google Scholar
  15. Bekhterev, V.M. (1903), Suggestion and its role in the social life. 2nd ed. St. Peters burg: K.L. Rikker.Google Scholar
  16. Bergson, H. (1911), Creative evolution. New York: Henry Holt & Co.Google Scholar
  17. Boodin, J.E. (1913), The existence of social minds. American Journal of Sociology 19, 1, 1–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cairns, R.B. (1979), Social development. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  19. Cairns, R.B. (1985), Sociogenesis and aggression: A modern perspective on the ontogeny and phytogeny of violence. Unpublished Manuscript.Google Scholar
  20. Cairns, R.B. (1992 in press), The making of a developmental science: The contributions and intellectual heritage of James Mark Baldwin.Developmental Psychology.Google Scholar
  21. Catán, L. (1986), The dynamic display of process: Historical development and contemporary uses of the microgenetic method. Human Development, 29, 252–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Cavalli-Sforza, L.L., Feldman, M.W., Chen, K.H., & Dornbusch, S.M. (1982), Theory and observation in cultural transmission.Science, 218, 19–27.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Chapman, M. (1988a), Constructive evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Chapman, M. (1988b), Contextuality and directionality of cognitive development. Human Development, 31, 92–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Cohen, Y.A. (1971), The shaping of men’s minds: adaptations to imperatives of culture. In M.L. Wax, S. Diamond & F.O. Gearing (eds.), Anthropological perspectives on education (pp. 19–50). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  26. Cole, M. (1992), Context, modularity and the cultural constitution of development. In L.T. Winegar & J. Valsiner (eds.), Children’s development within social context. Vol 2. Research and methodology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  27. Danziger, K. (1990), Constructing the subject. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Diaz, R.M., Neal, C.J., & Amaya-Williams, M. (1990), The social origins of self-regulation. In L.C. Moll (ed.), Vygotsky and education (pp. 127–154). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Doise, W. (1985), On the social development of the intellect. In V.L. Shulman, L.C.R. Restaino-Baumann, & L. Butler (eds.), The future of Piagetian theory: Neo-Piagetians (pp. 99–121). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  30. Elbers, E. (1986), Interaction and instruction in the conservation experiment. European Journal of Psychology of Education 1, 1, 77–89.Google Scholar
  31. Emerson, C. (1983), Bakhtin and Vygotsky on internalization of language.Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 5, 1, 9–12.Google Scholar
  32. Foote, N.N. (1957), Concept and method in the study of human development. In M. Sherif & M.O. Wilson (eds.), Emerging problems in social psychology. Series III (pp. 29–53). Norman, OK: University Book Exchange.Google Scholar
  33. Gergen, K. (1988), If persons are texts. In S.B. Messer, L.A. Sass, & R.L. Woolfolk (eds.), Hermeneutics and psychological theory (pp. 28–51). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Gergen, K. (1991), The communal creation of meaning. Paper presented at the Jean Piaget Society meeting, Philadelphia, PA.Google Scholar
  35. Hermans, H.J.M. (1991), The person as co-investigator in self-research: valuation theory. European Journal of Personality. 5, 217–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hermans, H.J.M., Bonarius, H. (1991a), The person as co-investigator in personality research.European Journal of Personality 5, 199–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hermans, H.J.M., Bonarius, H. (1991b), Static laws in a dynamic psychology? European Journal of Personality 5, 245–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Hughes, D.J. (1991), Blending with another: an analysis of trance channeling in the United States. Ethos 19, 2, 161–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Janet, P. (1925), Psychological healing. New York: MacMillan.Google Scholar
  40. Janet, P. (1926), De l’angoisse a l’extase. Paris: Félix Alean.Google Scholar
  41. Janet, P. (1930), Pierre Janet. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A history of psychology in autobiography. Vol. 1 (pp. 123–133). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. John-Steiner, V., & Tatter, P. (1983), An interactionist model of language development. In B. Bain (Ed.), The sociogenesis of language and human conduct (pp. 79–97). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  43. Kindermann, T., & Valsiner, J. (1989), Research strategies in culture-inclusive developmental psychology. In J. Valsiner (ed.), Child development in cultural context (pp. 13–50). Toronto: Hogrefe & Huber.Google Scholar
  44. Kojima, H. (1988), The role of belief-value systems related to child-rearing and education: The case of early modern to modern Japan. In D. Sinha & H.S. Kao (eds.), Social values and development (pp. 227–253). New Delhi: Sage.Google Scholar
  45. Kozulin, A. (1990), Vygotsky’s psychology: A biography of ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  46. Kreppner, K. (1992 in press). William L. Stern. Developmental Psychology.Google Scholar
  47. Lave, J. (1988), Cognition in practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Lawrence, J.A., & Valsiner, J. (1992 in press), Social determinacy of human development: An analysis of the conceptual roots of the internalization process. Human Development.Google Scholar
  49. Lightfoot, C, & Folds-Bennett, T. (1992), Description and explanation in developmental research: separate agendas. In J. Asendorpf & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Stability and change in development: A study of methodological reasoning (pp. 207–228). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  50. Marková, I. (1987), On the interaction of opposites in psychological processes. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 17, 279–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Marková, I. (1990a), The development of self-consciousness: Baldwin, Mead, and Vygotsky, In J.E. Faulconer & R.N. Williams (eds.), Reconsidering psychology: Perspectives from Continental philosophy (pp. 151–174). Pittsburgh, PA: Duquesne University Press.Google Scholar
  52. Marková, I. (1990b), On three principles of human social development. In G. Butter worth & P. Bryant (eds.), Causes of development (pp. 186–211). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  53. Mead, G.H. (1903), The definition of the psychical. Decennial Publications of the University of Chicago, 3 (Ser. 1), 77–112.Google Scholar
  54. Mead, G.H. (1910), What social objects must psychology presuppose? Journal of Psychology, 7 174–180.Google Scholar
  55. Mead, G.H. (1934), Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  56. Mead, M. (1963), Socialization and enculturation. Current Anthropology, 4, 2, 184–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Obeyesekere, G. (1990), The work of culture: symbolic transformation in psychoanalysis and anthropology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  58. Oliver, S.C. (1965), Individuality, freedom of choice, and cultural flexibility of the Kamba. American Anthropologist 67, 421–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Paulhan, F. (1928), Qu’est-ce que le sens des mots. Journal de Psychologie, 25, 289–329.Google Scholar
  60. Piaget, J. (1952), Jean Piaget. In E.G. Boring, H. Werner, H.S. Langfeld & R.M. Yerkes (eds.), A history of psychology in autobiography. Vol 4 (pp. 237–256). Worcester, Ma.: Clark University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Piaget, J. (1965), Psychology and philosophy. In B.B. Wolman & E. Nagel (eds.), Scientific psychology: Principles and approaches (pp. 28–43.). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  62. Piaget, J. (1971), Biology and knowledge. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  63. Piaget, J. (1977), The role of action in the development of thinking. In W.E. Overton & J. McGallagher (eds.), Knowledge and development. Vol. 1. Advances in research and theory (pp. 17–42). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  64. Rheingold, H.R. (1985), Development as the acquisition of familiarity. Annual Review of Psychology 36, 1–17.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Robinson, J.A. (1988), “What we’ve got here is a failure to communicate”: The cultural context of meaning. In J. Valsiner (ed.), Child development within culturally structured environments. Vol. 2. Social co-construction and environmental guidance of development (pp. 137–198). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  66. Rogoff, B. (1990), Apprenticeship in thinking. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  67. Rommetveit, R. (1979), On common codes and dynamic residuals in human communication. In R. Rommetveit & R. Blakar (eds.), Studies of language, thought and verbal communication (pp. 163–175). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  68. Royce, J. (1894), The imitative functions, and their place in human nature. Century Magazine, 26, 137–145.Google Scholar
  69. Semin, G. (1989), On genetic social psychology: A rejoinder to Doise.European Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 401–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Sherif, M. (1935), A study of some social factors in perception. Archives of Psychology No. 187, 1–60.Google Scholar
  71. Sherif, M. (1936), The psychology of social norms. New York: Harper & Brothers.Google Scholar
  72. Sherif, M. (1937), An experimental approach to the study of attitudes. Sociometry 1, 1–2, 90–98.Google Scholar
  73. Shore, B. (1991), Twice-born, once conceived: meaning construction and cultural cognition. American Anthropologist 93, 1, 9–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Shotter, J. (1990), Knowing of the third kind. Utrecht: ISOR.Google Scholar
  75. Siegler, R.S., & Crowley, K. (1991), The microgenetic method: A direct means for studying cognitive development. American Psychologist 46, 6, 606–620.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Sigel, I.E., & Cocking, R.R. (1977), Cognition and communication: a dialectic paradigm for development. In M. Lewis & L.A. Rosenblum (eds.), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language (pp. 207–226). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  77. Simmel, G. (1906), The sociology of secrecy and of secret societies. American Journalof Sociology 11, 4, 441–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Spindler, G.D. (1974), The transmission of culture. In G.D. Spindler (ed.), Education and cultural process (pp. 279–310). New York: Holt, Rinehardt & Winston.Google Scholar
  79. Stern, W. (1938), General Psychology From the Personalist Standpoint. New York: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Valsiner. J. (1987), Culture and the development of children’s action. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  81. Valsiner, J. (1988), Ontogeny of co-construction of culture within socially organized environmental settings. In J. Valsiner (ed.), Child Development within Culturally Structured Environments. Vol. 2. Social Co-construction and Environmental Guidance of Development (pp. 283–297). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  82. Valsiner, J. (1989a), Human development and culture. Lexington, MA: D.C. Heath.Google Scholar
  83. Valsiner, J. (1989b), Collective coordination of progressive empowerment. In L.T. Winegar (ed.), Social interaction and the development of children’s understanding (pp. 7–20). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  84. Valsiner, J. (1991), Building theoretical bridges over a lagoon of everyday events. Human Development, 34, 307–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Valsiner, J. (1992a), Introduction: Social co-construction of psychological development from a comparative-cultural perspective. In J. Valsiner (ed.), Child development within culturally structured environments. Vol. 3. Comparative-cultural and constructivist perspectives. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  86. Valsiner, J. (1992b),. Epilogue: Comparative-cultural co-constructionism and its discontents. In J. Valsiner (ed.), Child development within culturally structured environments. Vol. 3. Comparative-cultural and constructivist perspectives. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  87. Valsiner, J. (1992c), Social organization of cognitive development: internalization and externalization of constraint systems. In A. Demetriou and E. Efklides (eds.), The modern theories of cognitive development go to school. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  88. Valsiner, J. (1993 in press), Making of the future: temporality and the constructive nature of human development. In G. Turkewitz & D. Devenney (eds.), Timing as initial condition of development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  89. Valsiner, J., & Cairns, R.B. (1992 in press), Theoretical perspectives on conflict and development. In C.U. Shantz & W.W. Hartup (eds.), Conflict in child and adolescent development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  90. Valsiner, J., & Van der Veer, R. (1988), On the social nature of human cognition: An analysis of the shared intellectual roots of George Herbert Mead and Lev Vygotsky.Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 18, 117–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1988), Lev Vygotsky and Pierre Janet: On the origin of the concept of sociogenesis. Developmental Review, 8, 52–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1991), Understanding Vygotsky: A quest for synthesis. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  93. Van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1992 in press), Sociogenetic perspectives in the work of Pierre Janet. Storia e Critica della Psicologia Google Scholar
  94. Van Geert, P. (1988). The concept of transition in developmental theories. In W.J. Baker, L.P. Mos, H.V. Rappard & H.J. Stam (eds.), Recent trends in theoretical psychology (pp. 225–235). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Van Oers, B. (1988), Activity, semiotics and the development of children.Comenius No. 32, pp. 398–406.Google Scholar
  96. Vygotsky, L.S. (1934), Thinking and speech. Moscow-Leningrad: Gosudarstvennoe Sotsialno-eknomicheskoe Izdatel’ stvo.Google Scholar
  97. Vygotsky, L.S. (1933/1976), Play and its role in the mental development of the child. In J. Bruner, A. Jolly & K. Sylva (eds.), Play (pp.537–554). Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
  98. Vygotsky, L.S. (1987), The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky. Vol. 1. Problems of general psychology. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  99. Werner, H. (1938), William Stern’s personalisties and psychology of personality. Character and Personality, 7, 109–125.Google Scholar
  100. Werner, H. (1957), The concept of development from a comparative and organismic point of view. In D.B. Harris (ed.), The concept of development (pp. 125–147). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
  101. Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1984), Symbol formation. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  102. Wertsch, J.V. (1990), The voice of rationality in a sociocultural approach to mind. In L.C. Moll (ed.), Vygotsky and education (pp. 111–126). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  103. Wertsch, J.V. (1991), Voices in the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  104. Wertsch, J.V., & Stone, C.A. (1985), The concept of internalization in Vygotsky’s account of the genesis of higher mental functions. In J.V. Wertsch (ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives (pp. 162–179). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  105. Winegar, L.T. (1988), Children’s emerging understanding of social events: co-construction and social process. In J. Valsiner (ed.), Child development within culturally structured environments. Vol. 2. Social co-construction and environmental guidance of development (pp. 3–27). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  106. Winegar, L.T., Renninger, K.A., & Valsiner, J. (1989), Dependent-inde-pendence in adult-child relationships. In D.A. Kramer & M.J. Bopp (eds.), Transformation in clinical and developmental psychology (pp. 157–168). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag New York, Inc. 1994

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jaan Valsiner

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations