Skip to main content

Psychosocial Perspective on Cognitive Development: Construction of Adult-Child Intersubjectivity in Logic Tasks

  • Chapter
Sociogenesis Reexamined

Abstract

Obviously the concepts used in psychology at the beginning of this century were directly influenced by the then pervasive ideas of the theories of evolution. Looking back to this period it can be seen how such ideas gave a very important impulse to the study of human behavior. However several decades later, it would be well worth reviewing these concepts and either enlarging on them (as seemingly suggested by the organizers of the present symposium through the consideration of “sociogenesis”) or substituting new metaphors and concepts to the existing ones, in order to draw attention to aspects of psychological reality that the preceding perspective might cause to be neglected. This presentation takes as standpoint the crossroad of these two lines of thought. Indeed, in our opinion, suggesting new concepts (e.g., “intersubjectivity”) can be fruitful for the advance of science mostly if it can simultaneously account for both the already known phenomena and for newly described processes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Bearison, D.J. S. Magzamen & E.K. Filardo (1986), Socio-cognitive growth in young children. Merill-Palmer Quarterly, 32, 51–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, N. (1986), Analysis of post-experimental interviews with kindergarten children concerning the Piagetian test of the conservation of the liquids. University of Neuchâtel (Switzerland), Séminaire de Psychologie.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaye, A. (1988), Mécanismes générateurs de progrès lors de la résolution à deux d’un produit de deux ensembles par des enfants de 5–6 ans. In A.N. Perret-Clermont & M. Nicolet (eds.), Interagir et connaître, Cousset, Switzerland: Delval, pp.41–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bovet, M., S. Parrat-Dayan, & D. Deshusses-Addor (1981), Peut-on parler de précocité et de régression dans la conservation? I. Précocité. Archives de Psychologie, 49, 191, 289–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carraher, T.N. (1989), Negotiating the results of mathematical computations. International Journal of Educational Research, 13, 6, 637–646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Paolis, P. & V. Girotto (1987), Social marking of cognitive operations. The effect of different social rules. European Journal of Psychology of Education, II, 3, 219–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doise, W. & G. Mugny (1984), The social development of intellect. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doise, W., S. Dionnet & G. Mugny (1975), Conflit socio-cognitif, marquage social et développement cognitif. Cahiers de Psychologie, 21, 231–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doise, W., G. Mugny, & A.N. Perret-Clermont, A.N. (1975), Social interaction and the development of cognitive operations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 5, 367–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, M. (1978), Children’s mind, Glasgow: Fontana.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbers, E. (1986), Interaction and instruction in the conservation experiment. European Journal of the Psychology of Education, I, 1, 77–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, S. & B. Rogoff (1982), The strategies and efficacy of child versus teachers. Child Development, 53, 730–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emler, N. & G. Valiant (1982), Social interaction and cognitive conflict in thedevelopment of spatial coordination. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 295–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forman, E.A. (1987), Learning through peer interaction: a Vygotskian perspective. The Genetic Epistemologist, 15, 6–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forman, E.A. & M.J. Kraker (1985), The social origin of logic: the contributions of Piaget and Vygotsky. In M.W. Berkowitz (ed.), Peer conflict and psychological growth. New directions of child development, Vol. 29. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilly, M. & J.P. Roux (1984), Efficacité comparée du travail individuel et du travail en interaction socio-cognitive dans l’appropriation et la mise en oeuvre d’une procédure de résolution chez des enfants de 11–12 ans. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 4, 171–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossen, M. (1988), L’intersubjectivité en situation de test. Cousset, Switzerland: Delval.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossen, M. & N. Bell (1988), Définition de la situation de tesit et élaboration d’une notion logique. In A.N. Perret-Clermont & M. Nicolet (eds.), Interagir et connaître. Cousset, Switzerland: Delval.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hundeide, K. (1988), Metacontracts for situational definitions and for presentation of cognitive skills. The Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 10, 3, 85–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Light, P. (1986), Context, conservation and conversation. In M. Richards & P. Light (eds.), Children of social worlds: Development in social context. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press, pp. 170–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Light, P., T. Foot, C. Col bourn & I. McClelland (1987), Collaborative interactions at the microcomputer keyboard. Educational Psychology, 7, 13–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Light, P. & A.N. Perret-Clermont (1989), Social context effects in learning and testing. In A. Gellatly, D. Rogers & J.A. Sloboda (eds.), Cognition and Social Worlds. Oxford: Oxford Science Publication, pp. 99–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monteil, J.M. (1988), Comparaison sociale. Stratégies individuelles et médiations socio-cognitives. Un effet de différenciations comportementales dans le champ scolaire. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 111, 1, 3–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolet, M. & M. Grossen (1988), Testons-nous des compétences cognitives? Contribution psychosociologique à la situation de test à travers l’étude de conduites aux épreuves opératoires piagétiennes. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, I, 1, 72–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolet, M. & A. Iannaccone (1988), Norme sociale d’équité et contexte relationnel dans l’étude du marquage social. In A.N. Perret-Clermont & M. Nicolet (eds.), Interagir et connaître, Delval, Switzerland: Cousset, pp. 139–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perret-Clermont, A.N. (1980), Social interaction and cognitive development in children. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perret-Clermont, A.N. & M.L. Schubauer-Leoni (1981), Conflict and cooperation as opportunities for learning. In P. Robinson (ed.), Communication in development. London: Academic Press, pp. 203–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1971), Biology and knowledge, Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1990), Apprenticeship in thinking. Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1979), On common codes and dynamic residuals in human communication. In R.M. Blakar & R. Rommetveit (eds.), Studies of language, thought and verbal communication. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1985), Language acquisition as increasing linguistic structuring of experience and symbolic behaviour control. In J.V. Wertsch (ed.), Culture, communication and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roux, J.P. & M. Gilly (1984), Aide apportée par le marquage social dans une procédure de résolution chez des enfants de 12–13 ans: données et réflexions sur les mécanismes. Bulletin de Psychologie, XXXVII, 368, 145–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Säljö, R. & J. Wyndhamn (1987), The formal setting as context for cognitive activities. An empirical study of arithmetic operations under conflicting premisses for communication. European Journal of Psychology of Education, II, 3, 247–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubauer-Leoni, M.L. (1986), Maître-élève-savoir: analyse psychosociale du jeu et des enjeux de la relation didactique. Doctoral dissertation. University of Geneva (Switzerland).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubauer-Leoni, M.L., N. Bell, M. Grossen, & A.N. Perret-Clermont (1989), Problems of assessment of learning: the social construction of questions and answers in the scholastic context. International Journal of Educational Research, special issue on “Social factors in learning and instruction,” 13, 6, 671–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taal, M. & L. Oppenheimer (1989), Socio-cognitive conflict and peer interaction: development of compensation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 77–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (1984), Construction of the zone of proximal development in adult-child joint action. In B. Rogoff & J.V. Wertsch (eds.), Children’s learning in the zone of proximal development, San Fransisco: Jossey Bass, pp. 65–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J.V., N. Minick & F.J. Arns (1984), The creation of context in joint problem solving. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (eds.), Everyday cognition: its development in social context, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J.V. (1984), The zone of proximal development: some conceptual issues. In B. Rogoff & J.V. Wertsch (eds.), Children’s learning in the zone of proximal development, New directions for child development 23, San Francisco: Jossey Bass, pp. 7–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (1962), Thought and language, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, R.M. (1988), Normes égalitaires, conduites sociales de partage et acquisition de la conservation des quantités. In A.N. Perret-Clermont & M. Nicolet, Interagir et connaître, Cousset, Switerland: Delval.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1994 Springer-Verlag New York, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Grossen, M., Perret-Clermont, AN. (1994). Psychosocial Perspective on Cognitive Development: Construction of Adult-Child Intersubjectivity in Logic Tasks. In: de Graaf, W., Maier, R. (eds) Sociogenesis Reexamined. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2654-3_14

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2654-3_14

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-7622-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-2654-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics