Advertisement

What do We Plan or Control When We Perform a Voluntary Movement?

  • Gerald L. Gottlieb
Chapter

Abstract

The quantitative analysis of multiple degree of freedom movements is a relatively recent practice in motor control. In the early 1980s, Morasso, Lacquaniti, and Soechting published studies of arm reaching that identified certain distinctive kinematic characteristics (Morasso 1981; Soechting and Lacquaniti 1981; Lacquaniti et al. 1982). Morasso noted (p. 224) that “the common features among the different reaching movements are the single-peaked shape of the hand tangential velocity and the [straight] shape of the hand trajectory.” Soechting and Lacquaniti further noted that these properties were unaffected by changes in the load held in the hand or by the intended speed of movement. These properties of straightness and “bell-shaped” velocity profiles have become defining features of unconstrained human reaching movements, even though Hollerbach (1982) noted that movements in the sagittal plane tended to be more curved than those in the horizontal plane.

Keywords

Equilibrium Point Voluntary Movement Joint Torque Central Command Muscle Torque 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Almeida, G.L., Hong, D.H., Corcos, D.M., and Gottlieb, G.L. (1995). Organizing principles for voluntary movement: extending single joint rules. J. Neurophysiol., 74(4):1374–1381.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bastian, A.J., Martin, T.A., Keating, J.G., and Thach, W.T. (1996). Cerebellar ataxia: abnormal control of interaction torques across multiple joints. J. Neurophysiol. Google Scholar
  3. Feldman, A.G. and Levin, M.F. (1995). The origin and use of positional frames of reference in motor control. Behav. Brain Sci., 18:723–806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Flanagan, J.R. and Rao, A.K. (1995). Trajectory adaptation to a nonlinear visuomotor transformation: evidence of motion planning in visually perceived space. J. Neurophysiol., 74(5):2174–2178.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Gomi, H. and Kawato, M. (1996). Equilbrium-point control hypothesis examined by measured arm stiffness during multijoint movement. Science, 272:117–120.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Gottlieb, G.L. (1993). A computational model of the simplest motor program. J. Mot. Behav., 25(3):153–161.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Gottlieb, G.L. (1995). Shifting frames of reference but the same old point of view. Behav. Brain Sci., 18:758–759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Gottlieb, G.L. (1996). On the voluntary movement of compliant (inertial-viscoelastic) loads by parcellated control mechanisms. J. Neurophysiol., 76(5); (in press).Google Scholar
  9. Gottlieb, G.L., Song, Q., Hong, D., Almeida, G.L., and Corcos, D.M. (1996a). Coordinating movement at two joints: a principal of linear covariance. J. Neurophysiol., 75(4):1160–1764.Google Scholar
  10. Gottlieb, G.L., Song, Q., Hong, D., and Corcos, D.M. (1996b). Coordinating two degrees of freedom during human arm movement: load and speed invariance of relative joint torques. J. Neurophysiol., 76(5):3196–3206.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Gottlieb, G.L., Song, Q., Almeida, G.L., Hong, D., and Corcos, D.M. (1997). Directional control of planar human arm movement. J. Neurophysiol., 78:2985–2998.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Hogan, N. (1984). An organizing principal for a class of voluntary movements. J. Neurosci., 11:2745–2754.Google Scholar
  13. Hollerbach, J.M. (1982). Computers, brains and the control of movement. TINS, 5:189–192.Google Scholar
  14. Lacquaniti, F., Soechting, J.F., and Terzuolo, C.A. (1982). Some factors pertinent to the organization and control of arm movements. Brain Res., 252:394–397.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Latash, M.L. (1992). Independent control of joint stiffness in the framework of the equilibrium-point hypothesis. Biol. Cybern., 67:377–384.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Latash, M.L. and Gottlieb, G.L. (1991a). An equilibrium-point model for fast single-joint movement. I. Emergence of strategy-dependent EMG patterns. J. Mot. Behav., 23:163–178.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Latash, M.L. and Gottlieb, G.L. (1991b). Reconstruction of shifting elbow joint compliant characteristics during fast and slow voluntary movement. Neuroscience, 43(2/3):697–712.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Latash, M.L. and Zatsiorsky, V.M. (1993). Joint stiffness: myth or reality? Hum. Move. Sci., 12:653–692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Morasso, P. (1981). Spatial control of arm movements. Exp. Brain Res., 42:223–227.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Sainburg, R.L., Ghilardi, M.F., Poizner, H., and Ghez, C. (1995). Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. J. Neurophysiol., 73(2):820–835.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Shadmehr, R., Mussa-Ivaldi, F.A., and Bizzi, E. (1993). Postural force fields of the human arm and their role in generating multijoint movments. J. Neurosci., 13(1):45–62.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Soechting, J.F. and Lacquaniti, F. (1981). Invariant characteristics of a pointing movement in man. J. Neurosci., 1(7):710–720.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. St-Onge, N., Qi, H., and Feldman, A.G. (1993). The patterns of control signal underlying elbow joint movements in humans. Neurosci. Lett., 164:171–174.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Uno, Y., Kawato, M., and Suzuki, R. (1989). Formation and control of optimum trajectory in human multijoint arm movement—minimum torque-change model. Biol. Cybern., 61:89–101.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Wolpert, D.M., Ghahramani, Z., and Jordan, M.I. Perceptual distortion contributes to the curvature of human reaching movements. (1994). Exp. Brain Res., 98:153–156.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag New York, Inc. 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gerald L. Gottlieb

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations