Observational Learning and Language Acquisition: Principles of Learning, Systems, and Tasks

  • Grover J. Whitehurst
  • Barbara D. DeBaryshe
Part of the Springer Series in Language and Communication book series (SSLAN, volume 24)

Abstract

The thesis of this chapter is that language, like many other complex skills, is acquired in part through the process of observational learning. In observational learning, one person’s behavior (the model’s) is witnessed by a second person (the observer). Observational learning has occurred when some aspect of the model’s behavior comes to control a related aspect of the second person’s behavior. The observer’s behavior may match the model’s along the dimensions of topography, function, or discriminative context (Whitehurst, 1978). A topographical relation involves similarity of form. For example, a mother might say to her child, “You are incorrigible today;’ and the child replies, ”I’m’corrigbal.“ Matching along the functional dimension involves similarity between the outcome of the model’s and observer’s behavior. For example, a child might see people on a television commercial drinking bottles of soda; the child may then hurry into the kitchen to get a cup of juice. An example of observational learning of discriminative context (and topography) may be seen when a child is able to correctly name a real helicopter after having been exposed to pictures of helicopters and the wordhelicopterduring picture-book reading with parents.

Keywords

Expense Posit Tate Dition Dinate 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bandura, A. (1977).Social learning theory.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-all.Google Scholar
  2. Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1987). Competition, variation, and language learning. InB.MacWhinney (Ed.)Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp.157–193). Hillsdale,NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  3. Bell, R.Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization.Psychological Review 7581–95Google Scholar
  4. Bohannon, J.N., III, & Marquis, A.L.(1977). Children’s control of adult speech.Child Development 481002–1008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brown, R. (1973).A first language: The early stages.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Brown, R., & Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. In J.R. Hayes (Ed.)Cognition and the development of language (pp.155–207). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  7. Chomsky, N. (1959). [Review of B.F. Skinner’sVerbal Behavior]. Language 3526–58.Google Scholar
  8. Cuvo, A.J., & Riva, M.T. (1980). Generalization and transfer between comprehension and production: A comparison of retarded and nonretarded persons.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 13315–331.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. DeBaryshe, B.D., & Whitehurst, G.J. (1986). Intraverbal acquisition of semantic concepts by preschoolers.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 42169–186.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dollaghan, C. (1985). Child meets word: “Fast mapping” in preschool children.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 28449–454.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Falco, F.L. (1986).Social linguistic determinants of language development and delay.Unpublished doctoral dissertation. State University of New York at Stony Brook.Google Scholar
  12. Ravel!, J.H. (1981). Cognitive monitoring. In W.P. Dickson (Ed.)Children’s oral com-munication skills(pp. 35–60). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  13. Fraser, C., Bellugi, U., & Brown, R. (1963). Control of grammar in imitation, comprehension, and production.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior2, 121–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gardner, R.A., & Gardner, B.T. (1988). Feedforward vs feedbackward: An ethological alternative to the law of effect.Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11429–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gelman, S.A. & Markman, E.M. (1986). Categories and induction in young children.Cognition 23183–209.Google Scholar
  16. Goldin-Meadow, S. (1982). The resilience of recursion: A study of a communication system developed without a conventional language model. In E. Wanner & L. Gleitman (Eds.)Language acquisition: The state of the art.Cambridge; MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Goldin-Meadow, S., & Feldman, H. (1977). The development of language-like communication without a language model.Science 197401–403.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Harris, P. (1975). Inferences and semantic development.Journal of Child Language 2143–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hirsh-Pasek, K., Treiman, R., & Schneiderman, M. (1984). Brown and Hanlon revisited: Mother’s sensitivity to grammatical forms.Journal of Child Language 1181–88.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kamin, L.J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention and conditioning. In B.A. Campbell & R.M. Church (Eds.)Punishment and aversive behavior(pp. 279–296). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  21. Kaye, K. (1979). The development of skills. In G.J. Whitehurst & B.J. Zimmerman (Eds.)The functions of language and cognition(pp. 23–55). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  22. Leonard, L.B., & Schwartz, R.G. (1985). Early linguistic development of children with specific language impairment. In K.E. Nelson (Ed.)Children’s language(Vol. 5, pp. 291–318). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  23. Lovell, K., & Dixon, E.M. (1967). The growth of the control of grammar in imitation,comprehension, and production.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 831–39.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Macnamara, J. (1972). Cognitive basis of language learning in infants.Psychological Review79, 1–13.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Maratsos, M. (1983). Some current issues in the study of the acquisition of grammar. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.)Handbook of child psychology(4th ed., pp. 707–786). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  26. Masur, E.F. (1982). Mothers’ responses to infants’ object related gestures: Influences on lexical development.Journal of Child Language 9223–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. McCarthy, D. (1954). Language development in children. In L. Carmichael (Ed.)Manual of child psychology(2nd ed., pp. 492–630). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  28. Moerk, E.L. (1986). Environmental factors in early language acquisition. In G.J. White-hurst (Ed.)Annals of child development(Vol. 3, pp. 191–236). Greenwich, CT:JAI Press.Google Scholar
  29. Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38(Serial Nos. 1–2).Google Scholar
  30. Nelson, K., Rescorla, L., Grundel, J.M., & Benedict, H. (1978). Early lexicons: What do they mean?Child Development 49960–968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Nelson, K.E. (1982). Experimental gambits in the service of language-acquisition theory: From the Fiffin project to operation input swap. In S. Kuczaj (Ed.)Language develop-ment. Vol. 1: Syntax and semantics(pp. 159–199). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  32. Ninio, A. (1983). Joint book reading as a multiple vocabulary acquisition device.Develop-mental Psychology 19445–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Pavlov, I.P. (1927).Conditioned reflexes.Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Penner, S.G. (1987). Parental responses to grammatical and ungrammatical utterances.Child Development 58376–384.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Rescorla, R.A. (1980).Pavlovian second-order conditioning.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rondal, A.J. (1985).Adult-child interaction and the process of language acquisition.New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  36. Ruder, K.F., Hermann, P., & Schiefelbusch, R.L. (1977). Effects of verbal imitation and comprehension training on verbal production.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 659–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rumelhart, D.E., & McClelland, J.L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of verbs. In J.L.McClelland, D.E. Rumelhart, & the PDP Research Group (Eds.)Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Volume 2: Psychological and biological models(pp. 216–271). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  38. Skinner, B.F. (1957).Verbal Behavior.New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Snow, C.E. (1981). The uses of imitation.Journal of Child Language 8205–212.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Snow. C.E. (1983). Saying it again: The role of expanded and deferred imitations in lan-guage acquisition. In K.E. Nelson (Ed.)Child language(Vol. 4, pp. 29–58). New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
  41. Sokolov, E.N. (1960). Neuronal models and the orienting reflex. In M.A.B. Brazier (Ed.)The central nervous system and behavior.New York: Josiah Macy, Jr., Foundation.Google Scholar
  42. Stewart, D.M., & Hamilton, M.L. (1976). Imitation as a learning strategy in the acquisi-tion of vocabulary.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 21380–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Stine, E.L., & Bohannon, J.N., III (1983). Interaction, imitations, and language acquisi-tion.Journal of Child Language 10589–603.Google Scholar
  44. Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style.First Language 4197–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Valdez-Menchaca, M.C., & Whitehurst, G.J. (1987).The role of time delay in spontaneous language production.Unpublished manuscript. State University of New York at Stony Brook.Google Scholar
  46. Valdez-Menchaca, M.C., & Whitehurst, G.J. (1988). The effects of incidental teaching on vocabulary acquisition by young children.Child Development59, 1451–1459.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Werner, H., & Kaplan, E. (1952). The acquisition of word meanings: A developmental study.Monographs for the Society for Research on Child Development15 (Serial No. 51).Google Scholar
  48. Whitehurst, G.J. (1977). Comprehension, selective imitation, and the CIP hypothesis.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 2323–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Whitehurst, G.J. (1978). Observational learning. In A.C. Catania & T.A. Brigham (Eds.)Handbook of applied behavior analysis: Social and instructional processes (pp.142–178). New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
  50. Whitehurst, G.J. (1979). Meaning and semantics. In G.J. Whitehurst & B.J. Zimmerman (Eds.)The functions of language and cognition(pp.115–139). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  51. Whitehurst, G.J. (1982). Language development. In B.B. Wolman (Ed.)Handbook of developmental psychology (pp.367–386). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  52. Whitehurst, G.J. (1985, April). The role of imitation in language learning by children with language delay. In G.E. Speidel (Chair)The many faces of imitation.Symposium conducted at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
  53. Whitehurst, G.J., Falco, F.L., Fischel, J.E., Lonigan, C., Valdez-Menchaca, M.C., DeBaryshe, B.D., & Caulfield, M.B. (1989). Accelerating language development through picture book reading.Developmental Psychology 24552–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Whitehurst, G.J., & Fischel, J.E. (1988). Feedback in the acquisition of language and other complex behavior.Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11478–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Whitehurst, G.J., Fischel, J.E., Caulfield, M., DeBaryshe, B., & Valdez-Menchaca, M.C. (1989). Assessment and treatment of early expressive language delay. In P. Zelazo & R.Barr (Eds.)Challenges to developmental paradigms. Implications for theory assessment and treatment.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  56. Whitehurst, G.J., Fischel, J.E., Lonigan, C.J., Valdez-Menchaca, M.C., & DeBaryshe, B. D., & Caulfield, M.B. (1988). Verbal interaction in families of normal and expressive language delayed children.Developmental Psychology 24690–699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Whitehurst, G.J., Kedesdy, J., & White, T.G. (1982). A functional analysis of meaning. In S.A. Kuczaj (Ed.)Language development. Volume 1: Syntax and semantics (pp.397–427). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  58. Whitehurst, G.J., & Sonnenschein, S. (1985). The development of communication: A functional analysis. In G.J. Whitehurst (Ed.)Annals of child development (Vol.2). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
  59. Whitehurst, G.J., Sonnenschein, S., & Ianfolla, B.J. (1981). Learning to communicate from models: Children confuse length with information.Child Development 52507–513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Whitehurst, G.J., & Valdez-Menchaca, M.C. (1988). What is the role of reinforcement in early language acquisition?Child Development 59430–440.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Whitehurst, G.J., & Vasta, R. (1975). Is language acquired through imitation?Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 437–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • Grover J. Whitehurst
  • Barbara D. DeBaryshe

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations