Advertisement

Component Deficits of Neglect

  • Paolo Bartolomeo
Chapter

Abstract

Neglect behavior appears to be the complex resultant of a number of concurrent elementary deficits of different levels of severity and with different modes of interaction depending on the patient. This chapter outlines some of the putative elementary deficits of attention, motor preparation, and working memory that may contribute to neglect behavior.

Keywords

Attentional capture Disengagement deficit Response bias Directional hypokinesia Spatial working memory Nonlateralized deficits 

References

  1. Anderson B, Mennemeier M, Chatterjee A. Variability not ability: another basis for performance decrements in neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:785–96.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Arbib MA. Perceptual structures and distributed motor control. In: Brooks VB, editor. Handbook of physiology: the nervous system II. Motor control. Bethesda: American Physiological Society; 1981. p. 1449–80.Google Scholar
  3. Ballard DH. Animate vision. Artif Intell. 1991;48:57–86.Google Scholar
  4. Bartolomeo P. The novelty effect in recovered hemineglect. Cortex. 1997;33:323–32.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bartolomeo P. The traffic light paradigm: a reaction time task to study laterally directed arm movements. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc. 2002;9:32–40.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bartolomeo P. Visual neglect. Curr Opin Neurol. 2007;20:381–6.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bartolomeo P, Chokron S. Left unilateral neglect or right hyperattention? Neurology. 1999;53:2023–7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Bartolomeo P, Chokron S. Levels of impairment in unilateral neglect. In: Boller F, Grafman J, editors. Handbook of neuropsychology. 2nd ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers; 2001. p. 67–98.Google Scholar
  9. Bartolomeo P, Chokron S. Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;26:217–34.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bartolomeo P, D’Erme P, Gainotti G. The relationship between visuospatial and representational neglect. Neurology. 1994;44:1710–4.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Bartolomeo P, D’Erme P, Perri R, Gainotti G. Perception and action in hemispatial neglect. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36:227–37.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Bartolomeo P, Chokron S, Siéroff E. Facilitation instead of inhibition for repeated right-sided events in left neglect. Neuroreport. 1999;10:3353–7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Bartolomeo P, Siéroff E, Decaix C, Chokron S. Modulating the attentional bias in unilateral neglect: the effects of the strategic set. Exp Brain Res. 2001a;137:424–31.Google Scholar
  14. Bartolomeo P, Siéroff E, Chokron S, Decaix C. Variability of response times as a marker of diverted attention. Neuropsychologia. 2001b;39:358–63.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Bartolomeo P, Urbanski M, Chokron S, Chainay H, Moroni C, Siéroff E, Belin C, Halligan P. Neglected attention in apparent spatial compression. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:49–61.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Bartolomeo P, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Doricchi F. Left unilateral neglect as a disconnection syndrome. Cereb Cortex. 2007;45:3127–48.Google Scholar
  17. Barton JJ, Behrmann M, Black S. Ocular search during line bisection. The effects of hemi-neglect and hemianopia. Brain. 1998;121:1117–31.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Basso G, Nichelli P, Frassinetti F, di Pellegrino G. Time perception in a neglected space. Neuroreport. 1996;7:2111–4.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Battelli L, Pascual-Leone A, Cavanagh P. The ‘when’ pathway of the right parietal lobe. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:204–10.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Behrmann M, Moscovitch M. Object-centered neglect in patients with unilateral neglect: effects of left-right coordinates of objects. J Cogn Neurosci. 1994;6:1–16.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Behrmann M, Tipper SP. Attention accesses multiple reference frames: evidence from visual neglect. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1999;25:83–101.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Behrmann M, Black SE, Murji S. Spatial attention in the mental architecture: evidence from neuropsychology. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1995;17:220–42.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Bertelson P. The time course of prepration. Q J Exp Psychol. 1967;19:272–9.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Berti A, Frassinetti F, Umiltà C. Nonconscious reading? Evidence from neglect dyslexia. Cortex. 1994;30:181–97.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Bisiach E. Mental representation in unilateral neglect and related disorders. Q J Exp Psychol. 1993;46A:435–61.Google Scholar
  26. Bisiach E, Berti A, Vallar G. Analogical and logical disorders underlying unilateral neglect of space. In: Posner MI, Marin OS, editors. Attention and performance XI. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1985. p. 239–49.Google Scholar
  27. Bisiach E, Geminiani G, Berti A, Rusconi ML. Perceptual and premotor factors of unilateral neglect. Neurology. 1990;40:1278–81.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Bisiach E, Rusconi ML, Peretti VA, Vallar G. Challenging current accounts of unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia. 1994;32:1431–4.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Bisiach E, Tegnér R, Làdavas E, Rusconi ML, Mijovic’ D, Hjaltason H. Dissociation of ophtalmokinetic and melokinetic attention in unilateral neglect. Cereb Cortex. 1995;5:439–47.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Boly M, Balteau E, Schnakers C, Degueldre C, Moonen G, Luxen A, Phillips C, Peigneux P, Maquet P, Laureys S. Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somatosensory perception in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2007;104:12187–92.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Bottini G, Sterzi R, Vallar G. Directional hypokinesia in spatial hemineglect: a case study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55:562–5.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Bourgeois A, Chica AB, Migliaccio R, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Bartolomeo P. Cortical control of inhibition of return: evidence from patients with inferior parietal damage and visual neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:800–9.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Bourgeois A, Chica AB, Valero-Cabré A, Bartolomeo P. Cortical control of inhibition of return: causal evidence for task-dependent modulations by dorsal and ventral parietal regions. Cortex. 2013a;49:2229–38.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Bourgeois A, Chica AB, Valero-Cabré A, Bartolomeo P. Cortical control of inhibition of return: exploring the causal contributions of the left parietal cortex. Cortex. 2013b; doi:  10.1016/j.cortex.2013.08.004. [Epub ahead of print]
  35. Bueti D, Walsh V. The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364:1831–40.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Buschman TJ, Miller EK. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science. 2007;315:1860–2.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Butter CM, Rapcsak S, Watson RT, Heilman KM. Changes in sensory inattention, directional motor neglect and “release” of the fixation reflex following a unilateral frontal lesion: a case report. Neuropsychologia. 1988;26:533–45.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Callejas A, Lupiáñez J, Tudela P. The three attentional networks: on their independence and interactions. Brain Cogn. 2004;54:225–7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Caramazza A, Hillis AE. Spatial representation of words in the brain implied by studies of a unilateral neglect patient. Nature. 1990;346:267–9.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Carter AR, Astafiev SV, Lang CE, Connor LT, Rengachary J, Strube MJ, Pope DLW, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Resting interhemispheric functional magnetic resonance imaging connectivity predicts performance after stroke. Ann Neurol. 2010;67:365–75.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Chica AB, Lasaponara S, Lupiáñez J, Doricchi F, Bartolomeo P. Exogenous attention can capture perceptual consciousness: ERP and behavioural evidence. Neuroimage. 2010;51:1205–12.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Chica AB, Bartolomeo P, Valero-Cabre A. Dorsal and ventral parietal contributions to spatial orienting in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2011a;31:8143–9.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Chica AB, Lasaponara S, Chanes L, Valero-Cabré A, Doricchi F, Lupiáñez J, Bartolomeo P. Spatial attention and conscious perception: the role of endogenous and exogenous orienting. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2011b;73:1065–81.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Chica AB, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Toba MN, Malhotra P, Lupiáñez J, Bartolomeo P. Attention networks and their interactions after right-hemisphere damage. Cortex. 2012;48:654–63.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Corbetta M, Kincade MJ, Lewis C, Snyder AZ, Sapir A. Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1603–10.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Coslett HB, Bowers D, Fitzpatrick E, Haws B, Heilman KM. Directional hypokinesia and hemispatial inattention in neglect. Brain. 1990;113:475–86.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Coulthard E, Parton A, Husain M. The modular architecture of the neglect syndrome: implications for action control in visual neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1982–4.Google Scholar
  48. D’Erme P, Bartolomeo P. A unilateral defect of short-term visual memory in left hemineglect. Eur J Neurol. 1997;4:382–6.Google Scholar
  49. D’Erme P, Robertson I, Bartolomeo P, Daniele A, Gainotti G. Early rightwards orienting of attention on simple reaction time performance in patients with left-sided neglect. Neuropsychologia. 1992;30:989–1000.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. D’Erme P, Robertson IH, Bartolomeo P, Daniele A. Unilateral neglect: the fate of the extinguished visual stimuli. Behav Neurol. 1993;6:143–50.Google Scholar
  51. Daffner KR, Ahern GL, Weintraub S, Mesulam M-M. Dissociated neglect behaviour following sequential strokes in the right hemisphere. Ann Neurol. 1990;28:97–101.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Danckert J, Ferber S, Pun C, Broderick C, Striemer C, Rock S, Stewart D. Neglected time: impaired temporal perception of multisecond intervals in unilateral neglect. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:1706–20.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Doricchi F, Incoccia C, Galati G. Influence of figure-ground contrast on the implicit and explicit processing of line drawings in patients with left unilateral neglect. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1997;14:573–94.Google Scholar
  54. Dorris MC, Klein RM, Everling S, Munoz DP. Contribution of the primate superior colliculus to inhibition of return. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:1256–63.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Doty R. Forebrain commissures: glimpses of neurons producing mind. In: Zaidel E, Iacoboni M, editors. The parallel brain: the cognitive neuroscience of the corpus callosum. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 2003. p. 157–65.Google Scholar
  56. Driver J, Halligan PW. Can visual neglect operate in object-centered co-ordinates? An affirmative single-case study. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1991;8:475–96.Google Scholar
  57. Driver J, Pouget A. Object-centered visual neglect, or relative egocentric neglect? J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12:542–5.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Driver J, Baylis GC, Goodrich SJ, Rafal RD. Axis-based neglect of visual shapes. Neuropsychologia. 1994;32:1353–65.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Duhamel JR, Colby CL, Goldberg ME. The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. Science. 1992;255:90–2.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Eagleman DM, Tse PU, Buonomano D, Janssen P, Nobre AC, Holcombe AO. Time and the brain: how subjective time relates to neural time. J Neurosci. 2005;25:10369–71.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Egly R, Driver J, Rafal RD. Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion patients. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1994;123:161–77.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:340–7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Farah MJ, Brunn JL, Wong AB, Wallace MA, Carpenter PA. Frames of reference for allocating attention to space: evidence from the neglect syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 1990;28:335–47.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Findlay JM, Gilchrist ID. Active vision: the psychology of looking and seeing. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003.Google Scholar
  65. Fox MD, Raichle ME. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:700–11.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Gainotti G, Tiacci C. Patterns of drawing disability in right and left hemispheric patients. Neuropsychologia. 1970;8:379–84.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Gainotti G, Messerli P, Tissot R. Qualitative analysis of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to the laterality of cerebral lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1972;35:545–50.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Gainotti G, D’Erme P, Bartolomeo P. Early orientation of attention toward the half space ipsilateral to the lesion in patients with unilateral brain damage. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1991;54:1082–9.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Gainotti G, Bourlon C, Bartolomeo P. La négligence spatiale unilatérale. In: Lechevalier B, Viader F, Eustache F, editors. Traité de neuropsychologie clinique. Bruxelles/Paris: De Boeck – Inserm; 2008. p. 627–49.Google Scholar
  70. Gazzaniga MS, Baynes K. Consciousness, introspection, and the split-brain: the two minds/one body problem. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The new cognitive neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2000. p. 1355–63.Google Scholar
  71. Geschwind N. Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man – part II. Brain. 1965;88:585–644.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Gross J, Schmitz F, Schnitzler I, Kessler K, Shapiro K, Hommel B, Schnitzler A. Modulation of long-range neural synchrony reflects temporal limitations of visual attention in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:13050–5.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Habekost T, Rostrup E. Visual attention capacity after right hemisphere lesions. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1474–88.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Harrington DL, Haaland KY, Knight RT. Cortical networks underlying mechanisms of time perception. J Neurosci. 1998;18:1085–95.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Harvey M, Milner AD, Roberts RC. An investigation of hemispatial neglect using the landmark task. Brain Cogn. 1995;27:59–78.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Harvey M, Kramer-McCaffery T, Dow L, Murphy PJ, Gilchrist ID. Categorisation of ‘perceptual’ and ‘premotor’ neglect patients across different tasks: is there strong evidence for a dichotomy? Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:1387–95.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Coslett HB, Whelan H, Watson RT. Directional hypokinesia: prolonged reaction times for leftward movements in patients with right hemisphere lesions and neglect. Neurology. 1985;35:855–9.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Hier DB, Mondlock J, Caplan LR. Recovery of behavioral abnormalities after right hemisphere stroke. Neurology. 1983;33:345–50.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Hjaltason H, Tegner R, Tham K, Levander M, Ericson K. Sustained attention and awareness of disability in chronic neglect. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34:1229–33.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Husain M, Rorden C. Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:26–36.Google Scholar
  81. Husain M, Shapiro K, Martin J, Kennard C. Abnormal temporal dynamics of visual attention in spatial neglect patients. Nature. 1997;385:154–6.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Husain M, Mannan S, Hodgson T, Wojciulik E, Driver J, Kennard C. Impaired spatial working memory across saccades contributes to abnormal search in parietal neglect. Brain. 2001;124:941–52.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Ishiai S, Watabiki S, Lee E, Kanouchi T, Odajima N. Preserved leftward movement in left unilateral spatial neglect due to frontal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994a;57:1085–90.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Ishiai S, Sugushita M, Watabiki S, Nakayama T, Kotera M, Gono S. Improvement of left unilateral spatial neglect in a line extension task. Neurology. 1994b;44:294–8.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Ishiai S, Seki K, Koyama Y, Gono S. Ineffective leftward search in line bisection and mechanisms of left unilateral spatial neglect. J Neurol. 1996;243:381–7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Kinsbourne M. Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In: Robertson IH, Marshall JC, editors. Unilateral neglect: clinical and experimental studies. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1993. p. 63–86.Google Scholar
  87. Kleist K. Gehirnpathologie. Leipzig: Barth; 1934.Google Scholar
  88. Koch G, Oliveri M, Cheeran B, Ruge D, Lo Gerfo E, Salerno S, Torriero S, Marconi B, Mori F, Driver J, Rothwell JC, Caltagirone C. Hyperexcitability of parietal-motor functional connections in the intact left-hemisphere of patients with neglect. Brain. 2008;131:3147–55.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Làdavas E, Carletti M, Gori G. Automatic and voluntary orienting of attention in patients with visual neglect: horizontal and vertical dimensions. Neuropsychologia. 1994a;32:1195–208.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  90. Làdavas E, Farne A, Carletti M, Zeloni G. Neglect determined by the relative location of responses. Brain. 1994b;117:705–14.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Làdavas E, Zeloni G, Zaccara G, Gangemi P. Eye movements and orienting of attention in patients with visual neglect. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997;9:67–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Liu GT, Bolton AR, Price BH, Weintraub S. Dissociated perceptual-sensory and exploratory-motor neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55:701–6.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Losier BJ, Klein RM. A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal lobe damage. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25:1–13.Google Scholar
  94. Lupiáñez J, Decaix C, Siéroff E, Chokron S, Milliken B, Bartolomeo P. Independent effects of endogenous and exogenous spatial cueing: inhibition of return at endogenously attended target locations. Exp Brain Res. 2004;159:447–57.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Macquistan AD. Object-based allocation of visual attention in response to exogenous, but not endogenous, spatial precues. Psychon Bull Rev. 1997;4:512–5.Google Scholar
  96. Malhotra P, Jager HR, Parton A, Greenwood R, Playford ED, Brown MM, Driver J, Husain M. Spatial working memory capacity in unilateral neglect. Brain. 2005;128:424–35.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Malhotra P, Coulthard EJ, Husain M. Role of right posterior parietal cortex in maintaining attention to spatial locations over time. Brain. 2009;132:645–60.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Mannan SK, Mort DJ, Hodgson TL, Driver J, Kennard C, Husain M. Revisiting previously searched locations in visual neglect: role of right parietal and frontal lesions in misjudging old locations as new. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17:340–54.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Manning L, Kartsounis LD. Confabulations related to tacit awareness in visual neglect. Behav Neurol. 1993;6:211–3.Google Scholar
  100. Marshall JC, Halligan PW. Blindsight and insight into visuo-spatial neglect. Nature. 1988;336:766–7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. Marshall JC, Halligan P. Within- and between-task dissociations in visuo-spatial neglect: a case study. Cortex. 1995;31:367–76.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Mattingley JB, Driver J. Distinguishing sensory and motor deficits after parietal damage: an evaluation of reponse selection biases in unilateral neglect. In: Thier P, Karnath HO, editors. Parietal lobe contributions to orientation in 3D-space. Heidelberg: Springer; 1997. p. 309–37.Google Scholar
  103. Mattingley JB, Bradshaw JL, Phillips JG. Impairments of movement initiation and execution in unilateral neglect. Brain. 1992;115:1849–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Mattingley JB, Bradshaw JL, Bradshaw JA, Nettleton NC. Residual rightward attentional bias after apparent recovery from right hemisphere damage: implications for a multicomponent model of neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57:597–604.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. McGlinchey-Berroth R, Milberg W, Verfaellie M, Alexander M, Kiduff PT. Semantic processing in the neglected visual field: evidence from a lexical decision task. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1993;10:79–108.Google Scholar
  106. Mesulam MM. A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Ann Neurol. 1981;10:309–25.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  107. Miceli G, Capasso R. Word-centred neglect dyslexia: evidence from a new case. Neurocase. 2001;7:221–37.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Miglino O, Ponticorvo M, Bartolomeo P. Place cognition and active perception: a study with evolved robots. Connect Sci. 2009;21:3–14.Google Scholar
  109. Mijovic’ D. Mechanisms of visual spatial neglect: absence of directional hypokinesia in spatial exploration. Brain. 1991;114:1575–93.Google Scholar
  110. Morrow LA, Ratcliff G. The disengagement of covert attention and the neglect syndrome. Psychobiology. 1988;16:261–9.Google Scholar
  111. Müller HJ, Findlay JM. The effect of visual attention on peripheral discrimination thresholds in single and multiple element displays. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1988;69:129–55.Google Scholar
  112. Müller HJ, Rabbitt PM. Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1989;15:315–30.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  113. Na DL, Adair JC, Kang Y, Chung CS, Lee KH, Heilman KM. Motor perseverative behavior on a line cancellation task. Neurology. 1999;52:1569–76.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  114. Nakayama K, Mackeben M. Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention. Vision Res. 1989;29:1631–47.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  115. Natale E, Marzi CA, Bricolo E, Johannsen L, Karnath HO. Abnormally speeded saccades to ipsilesional targets in patients with spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:263–72.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. Nico D. Detecting directional hypokinesia: the epidiascope technique. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34:471–4.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  117. Oliveri M, Rossini PM, Traversa R, Cicinelli P, Filippi MM, Pasqualetti P, Tomaiuolo F, Caltagirone C. Left frontal transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces contralesional extinction in patients with unilateral right brain damage. Brain. 1999;122(Pt 9):1731–9.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Ota H, Fujii T, Suzuki K, Fukatsu R, Yamadori A. Dissociation of body-centered and stimulus-centered representations in unilateral neglect. Neurology. 2001;57:2064–2069.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  119. Pantano P, Di Piero V, Fieschi C, Judica A, Guariglia C, Pizzamiglio L. Pattern of CBF in the rehabilitation of visual spatial neglect. Int J Neurosci. 1992;66:153–61.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  120. Perani D, Vallar G, Paulesu E, Alberoni M, Fazio F. Left and right hemisphere contribution to recovery from neglect after right hemisphere damage – An [18F]FDG PET study of two cases. Neuropsychologia. 1993;31:115–25.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  121. Piercy M, Hécaen H, de Ajuriaguerra J. Constructional apraxia associated with unilateral cerebral lesions – left and right sided cases compared. Brain. 1960;83:225–42.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  122. Pisella L, Mattingley JB. The contribution of spatial remapping impairments to unilateral visual neglect. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;28:181–200.Google Scholar
  123. Pisella L, Alahyane N, Blangero A, Thery F, Blanc S, Pelisson D. Right-hemispheric dominance for visual remapping in humans. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;366:572–85.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  124. Posner MI. Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol. 1980;32:3–25.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  125. Posner MI, Cohen Y, Rafal RD. Neural systems control of spatial orienting. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1982;298:187–98.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  126. Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FJ, Rafal RD. Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. J Neurosci. 1984;4:1863–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  127. Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FA, Rafal RD. How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? Neuropsychologia. 1987;25:135–45.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  128. Pratt J, McAuliffe J. The effects of onsets and offsets on visual attention. Psychol Res. 2001;65:185–91.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Rafal RD, Posner MI, Friedman JH, Inhoff AW, Bernstein E. Orienting of visual attention in progressive supranuclear palsy. Brain. 1988;111:267–80.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  130. Rafal RD, Calabresi PA, Brennan CW, Sciolto TK. Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1989;15:673–85.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  131. Rastelli F, Funes MJ, Lupiáñez J, Duret C, Bartolomeo P. Left neglect: is the disengage deficit space- or object-based? Exp Brain Res. 2008;187:439–46.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Rastelli F, Tallon-Baudry C, Migliaccio R, Toba MN, Ducorps A, Pradat-Diehl P, Duret C, Dubois B, Valero-Cabré A, Bartolomeo P. Neural dynamics of neglected targets in patients with right hemisphere damage. Cortex. 2013;49:1989–96.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  133. Ress D, Backus BT, Heeger DJ. Activity in primary visual cortex predicts performance in a visual detection task. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:940–5.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  134. Riggio L, Kirsner K. The relationship between central cues and peripheral cues in covert visual orientation. Percept Psychophys. 1997;59:885–99.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  135. Ro T, Farné A, Chang E. Inhibition of return and the human frontal eye fields. Exp Brain Res. 2003;150:290–6.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  136. Robertson IH. Do we need the “lateral” in unilateral neglect? Spatially nonselective attention deficits in unilateral neglect and their implications for rehabilitation. Neuroimage. 2001;14:S85–90.Google Scholar
  137. Robertson IH, Mattingley JB, Rorden C, Driver J. Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness. Nature. 1998;395:169–72.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  138. Rossit S, Malhotra P, Muir K, Reeves I, Duncan G, Livingstone K, Jackson H, Hogg C, Castle P, Learmonth G, Harvey M. No neglect-specific deficits in reaching tasks. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:2616–24.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  139. Rusconi ML, Maravita A, Bottini G, Vallar G. Is the intact side really intact? Perseverative responses in patients with unilateral neglect: a productive manifestation. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:594–604.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  140. Rushmore RJ, Valero-Cabre A, Lomber SG, Hilgetag CC, Payne BR. Functional circuitry underlying visual neglect. Brain. 2006;129:1803–21.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  141. Russell C, Deidda C, Malhotra P, Crinion JT, Merola S, Husain M. A deficit of spatial remapping in constructional apraxia after right-hemisphere stroke. Brain. 2010;133:1239–51.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  142. Samuelsson H, Hjelmquist EK, Jensen C, Ekholm S, Blomstrand C. Nonlateralized attentional deficits: an important component behind persisting visuospatial neglect? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1998;20:73–88.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  143. Sapir A, Soroker N, Berger A, Henik A. Inhibition of return in spatial attention: direct evidence for collicular generation. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:1053–4.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  144. Sapir A, Kaplan JB, He BJ, Corbetta M. Anatomical correlates of directional hypokinesia in patients with hemispatial neglect. J Neurosci. 2007;27:4045–51.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  145. Seki K, Ishiai S, Koyama Y, Fujimoto Y. Appearance and disappearance of unilateral spatial neglect for an object: influence of attention-attracting peripheral stimuli. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34:819–26.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  146. Shepherd M, Findlay JM, Hockey RJ. The relationship between eye movements and spatial attention. Q J Exp Psychol. 1986;38A:475–791.Google Scholar
  147. Siéroff E, Decaix C, Chokron S, Bartolomeo P. Impaired orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect: a componential analysis. Neuropsychology. 2007;21:94–113.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  148. Small M, Ellis S. Brief remission periods in visuospatial neglect: evidence from long-term follow-up. Eur Neurol. 1994;34:147–54.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  149. Smania N, Martini MC, Gambina G, Tomelleri G, Palamara A, Natale E, Marzi CA. The spatial distribution of visual attention in hemineglect and extinction patients. Brain. 1998;121:1759–70.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  150. Snow JC, Mattingley JB. Goal-driven selective attention in patients with right hemisphere lesions: how intact is the ipsilesional field? Brain. 2006;129:168–81.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  151. Sprague JM. Interaction of cortex and superior colliculus in mediation of visually guided behavior in the cat. Science. 1966;153:1544–7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  152. Striemer C, Danckert J. Prism adaptation reduces the disengage deficit in right brain damage patients. Neuroreport. 2007;18:99–103.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  153. Tegnér R, Levander M. Through a looking glass. A new technique to demonstrate directional hypokinesia in unilateral neglect. Brain. 1991;114:1943–51.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  154. Triviño M, Correa A, Arnedo M, Lupiáñez J. Temporal orienting deficit after prefrontal damage. Brain. 2010;133:1173–85.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  155. Valdes-Sosa M, Bobes MA, Rodriguez V, Pinilla T. Switching attention without shifting the spotlight object-based attentional modulation of brain potentials. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998;10:137–51.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  156. Vivas AB, Humphreys GW, Fuentes LJ. Inhibitory processing following damage to the parietal lobe. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1531–40.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  157. Vivas AB, Humphreys GW, Fuentes LJ. Abnormal inhibition of return: a review and new data on patients with parietal lobe damage. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2006;23:1049–64.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  158. Volpe BT, Ledoux JE, Gazzaniga MS. Information processing of visual stimuli in an “extinguished” field. Nature. 1979;282:722–4.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  159. Vuilleumier P, Hester D, Assal G, Regli F. Unilateral spatial neglect recovery after sequential strokes. Neurology. 1996;46:184–9.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  160. Vuilleumier P, Sagiv N, Hazeltine E, Poldrack RA, Swick D, Rafal RD, Gabrieli JD. Neural fate of seen and unseen faces in visuospatial neglect: a combined event-related functional MRI and event-related potential study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:3495–500.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  161. Vuilleumier P, Sergent C, Schwartz S, Valenza N, Girardi M, Husain M, Driver J. Impaired perceptual memory of locations across gaze-shifts in patients with unilateral spatial neglect. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:1388–406.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  162. Wade DT, Wood VA, Hewer RL. Recovery of cognitive function soon after stroke: a study of visual neglect, attention span and verbal recall. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1988;51:10–3.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  163. Wilke M, Kagan I, Andersen RA. Functional imaging reveals rapid reorganization of cortical activity after parietal inactivation in monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012;109:8274–9.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  164. Wojciulik E, Husain M, Clarke K, Driver J. Spatial working memory deficit in unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:390–6.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  165. Wojciulik E, Rorden C, Clarke K, Husain M, Driver J. Group study of an “undercover” test for visuospatial neglect: invisible cancellation can reveal more neglect than standard cancellation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004;75:1356–8.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  166. Yantis S, Jonides J. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1990;16:121–34.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Further Reading

  1. Coulthard E, Parton A, Husain M. The modular architecture of the neglect syndrome: implications for action control in visual neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(8):1982–4.Google Scholar
  2. Danckert J. Spatial neglect: not simply disordered attention. In: Schweizer TA, Macdonald RL, editors. The behavioral consequences of stroke. New York: Springer; 2013. p. 71–94.Google Scholar
  3. Husain M, Rorden C. Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4(1):26–36.Google Scholar
  4. Losier BJ, Klein RM. A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal lobe damage. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25(1):1–13.Google Scholar
  5. Pisella L, Mattingley JB. The contribution of spatial remapping impairments to unilateral visual neglect. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;28(2):181–200.Google Scholar
  6. Robertson IH. Do we need the “lateral” in unilateral neglect? Spatially nonselective attention deficits in unilateral neglect and their implications for rehabilitation. Neuroimage. 2001;14(1):S85–90.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag London 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Paolo Bartolomeo
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Brain and Spine Institute Hôpital de la SalpêtrièreInstitut national de la santé et de la recherche médicale (INSERM)ParisFrance
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyCatholic UniversityMilanItaly

Personalised recommendations