Skip to main content

Active Symbols and Internal Models: Towards a Cognitive Connectionism

  • Chapter
Connectionism in Context

Part of the book series: Artificial Intelligence and Society ((HCS))

Abstract

There can be little doubt that the connectionist revolution is under way. In the last five years, the class of techniques that fall under the collective heading of “connectionism” has become the single most credible competitor to three decades of research in artificial intelligence (AI). Originally a response to the failures of the “symbolic” paradigm to live up to the expectations of the 1960s and 1970s, connectionist networks have shown promise in areas such as object recognition, pattern completion, speech synthesis and verb conjugation. However, these achievements do not, in and of themselves, constitute revolutionary advances in Al. Their real importance lies not so much in the results, but rather in the means used to achieve them.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Abbott V, Black J, Smith E (1985) The representation of scripts in memory. J Memory Lang 24: 179–199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barlow HB (1985) Cerebral cortex as model builder. In: Rose D, Dobson V (eds) Models of the visual cortex. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou L, Sewell D (1985) Contrasting the representation of scripts and categories. I Memory Lang 24: 646–665

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braitenberg V (1978) Cortical architectonics: general and areal. In: Brazier MA, Petsche H (eds) Architectonics of the cerebral cortex. Raven Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Braitenberg V (1984) Vehicles: experiments in synthetic psychology. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark A (1989) Microcognition: philosophy, cognitive science, and parallel distributed processing. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik KJW (1943) The nature of explanation. Cambridge University Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles JC (1957) The physiology of nerve cells. Johns Hopkins Press, Baltimore

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman G (1987) Neural Darwinism: the theory of neuronal group selection. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman JL (1990) Finding structure in time. Cognitive Sci 14: 179–211

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farah MJ (1988) Is visual imagery really visual? Overlooked evidence from neuropsychology. Psychol Rev 95: 307–317

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman JA (1988) Connectionist representation of concepts. In: Waltz D, Feldman JA (eds) Connectionist models and their implications: readings from cognitive science. Ablex, Norwood, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor JA, Pylyshyn ZW (1988) Connectionism and cognitive architecture: a critical analysis. Cognition 28: 3–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • French R, Weaver M (1987) The role of categories in the generation of counterfactuals: a connectionist interpretation. Proceedings of the Ninth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1941) A critical review of the concept of set in contemporary experimental psychology. Psychol Bull 38: 781–817

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg S (1987) Competitive learning: from interactive activation to adaptive resonance. Cognitive Sci 11: 23–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebb DO (1949) The Organization of Behavior. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstadter DR (1979) Gödel, Escher, Bach: an eternal golden braid. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume D (1737/1975) Of the idea of necessary connexions. In: An enquiry concerning human understanding and concerning the principles of morals. Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird PN (1981) Comprehension as the construction of mental models. Philos Trans R Soc London 295B: 353–374

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides J (1980) Towards a model of the minds eye movement. Can J Psychol 34: 103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D, Tversky A (1973) On the psychology of prediction. Psychol Rev 80: 237–251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan S (1970) The role of location processing in the perception of the environment. In: Archea J, Eastman C (eds) EDRA 2 ( Proceedings of the second annual environmental design research association conference ), Pittsburgh

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan S (1973) Cognitive maps in perception and thought. In: Downs RM, Stea D (eds) Image and environment. Aldine, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan S (1976) Adaptation, structure, and knowledge. In: Moore GT, Golledge RG (eds) Environmental knowing. Dowden, Hutchinson and Ross, Stroudsburg, PA

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan S (in press) Beyond rationality: clarity based decision making. In: Evans G, Garling T (eds) Environment, cognition, and action: an integrative, multidisciplinary approach. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 171–190

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan S, Kaplan R (1982) Cognition and environment. Praeger, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan S, Sonntag M, Chown E (1991) Tracing recurrent activity in cognitive elements (TRACE): a model of temporal dynamics in a cell assembly. Connection Sci 3: 179–206

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne M (1982) Hemispheric specialization and the growth of human understanding. Am Psycho 137:411–420

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinmuntz B (1968) The processing of clinical information by man and machine. In: Kleinmuntz B (ed) Formal representation of human judgement. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn S (1987) Seeing and imaging in the cerebral hemispheres: a computational approach. Psychol Rev 94: 148–175

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski A, Ajzen I (1983) Bias and error in human judgement. Euro J Social Psychol 13: 1–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lachman JL, Lachman R (1979) Theories of memory organization and human evolution. In: Puff CR (ed) Memory organization and structure. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachter J, Bever T (1988) The relationship between linguistic structure and associative theories of language learning: a constructive critique of some connectionist teaching models. Cognition 28: 195–247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff G, Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley KS (1950) In search of the engram. In: Society of Experimental Biology Symposium No. 4: Psychological mechanisms in animal behavior. Cambridge University Press, London, pp 478–505

    Google Scholar 

  • Levenick JR (1991) NAPS: a connectionist implementation of cognitive maps. Connection Sci 3: 107–126

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay RK (1988) Can this treatment raise the dead? Behav Brain Sci 10 (4)

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria AR (1973) The working brain. Penguin Books, Harmondsworth, pp 147–156

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch JC, Mountcastle VB, Talbot WH, Yin TCT (1977) Parietal lobe mechanisms for directed visual attention. J Neurophysiol 40: 362–389

    Google Scholar 

  • Macphail E (1987) The comparative psychology of intelligence. Behav Brain Sci 10 (4): 645–695

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Margolis H (1987) Patterns, thinking, and cognition. University of Chicago Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer D, Schvaneveldt R (1971) Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. I Exp Psychol: General 106: 227–234

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller G, Galanter E, Pribram K (1960) Plans and the structure of behavior. Henry Holt, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Miller N (1963) Some reflections on the law of effect produce a new alternative to drive reduction. In: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1963. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner PM (1957) The cell assembly: mark II. Psychol Rev 64: 242–252

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minsky M, Pap S (1969) Perceptrons. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett R, Ross L (1980) Human inference: strategies and shortcomings of social judgement of the Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman D (1980) Cognitive engineering and education. In: Tuma D, Reif F (eds) Problem solving and education. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford M, Chater N, Stenning K. (1990) Connectionism, classical cognitive science and experimental psychology. Al & Society 4: 73–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palm G (1982) Neural assemblies: an alternative approach to artificial intelligence. Springer-Verlag, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S, Prince A (1988) On language and connectionism: analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition 28: 73–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI (1973) Search strategies and problem solving. In: Cognition: an introduction. Scott Foresman, New York, pp 149–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn ZW (1980) Cognition and computation: issues in the foundations of cognitive science. Behav Brain Sci 3: 154–169

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rochester N, Holland JH, Haibt LH, Duda WL (1956) Tests on a cell assembly theory action of the brain, using a large digital computer. IRE Trans Information Theory, IT-2

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch E, Mervis C, Gray W, Johnson D, Boyes-Braem P (1976) Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychol 7: 573–605

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg C (1987) Revealing the structure of NETtalk’s internal representations. In: Proceedings of the Ninth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart D, McClelland J (1987) On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In: McClelland J, Rumelhart D (eds) Parallel distributed processing, Vol 2. Bradford, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Sejnowski T, Rosenberg C (1986) NETtalk: a parallel network that learns to read aloud (Technical Report 86/01). Johns Hopkins University, Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, Baltimore

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard R (1975) Form, formation and transformation of representations. In: Solso R (ed) Information processing and cognition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith E, Medin D (1981) Categories and concepts. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Smolensky P (1988) The constituent structure of mental states: a reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn. Technical report CU-CS-394-88, Department of Computer Science and Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado, Boulder

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonntag ML (1991) Learning sequences in an associative network: a step towards cognitive structure. Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor

    Google Scholar 

  • Stea D (1978) Environment, perception, and cognition: toward a model for mental maps. In: Kaplan S, Kaplan R (eds) Humanscape: environments for people. Duxbury, Belmont, CA

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman EC (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and man. Psychol Rev 55: 189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman EC, Brunswick E (1935) The organism and the causal texture of the environment. Psychol Rev 42: 43–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voss JF (1969) Associative learning and thought. In: Voss J (ed) Approaches to thought. Merrill, Columbus

    Google Scholar 

  • Voss JF, Tyler SW, Yengo LA (1983) Individual differences in the solving of social science problems. In: Dillon RF, Schmeck RR (eds) Individual differences in cognition. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason PC, Johnson-Laird PN (1972) Psychology of reasoning: structure and content. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinert F, Kluwe R (eds) (1987) Metacognition, motivation, and understanding. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams RJ, Zipser D (1988) A learning algorithm for continually running, fully-recurrent neural networks. Technical Report #8805, Institute of Cognitive Science, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla

    Google Scholar 

  • Wise SP, Desimone R (1988) Behavioral neurophysiology: insights into seeing and grasping. Science 242: 736–741

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 Springer-Verlag London Limited

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kaplan, S., Weaver, M., French, R.M. (1992). Active Symbols and Internal Models: Towards a Cognitive Connectionism. In: Clark, A., Lutz, R. (eds) Connectionism in Context. Artificial Intelligence and Society. Springer, London. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-1923-4_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-1923-4_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, London

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-540-19716-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4471-1923-4

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics