Advertisement

Postformal Thought: Commons’ Model

  • Gerald Young
Chapter

Abstract

Commons (2008a) has developed the most comprehensive model of postformal thought in the Neo-Piagetian literature. In 2008, he presented his model with colleagues in a special issue of the journal World Futures. The present chapter is largely based on the articles in the issue. After reviewing each article, I provide commentary. Commons (2008a) model involves four postformal stages, and mine consists of one stage with five substages. The major difference in the models is that Commons appears to have missed a step involving hierarchization after the first step in his series. In the following, I show how the descriptions that Commons provides for his four postformal steps and the examples given to elaborate them suggest that a hierarchical step is needed in his model and that it fits very well into his series. Moreover, I present several models that are comparable to those of Commons and colleagues. However, these models avoid some of the issues deriving from the Commons’ model that I point out in the comparison of the two models. In particular, I describe a model of management styles derived from the present model.

Keywords

Cognitive Development Management Style Collective Intelligence Formal Thought African Grey Parrot 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Boccia, M., & Campos, J. J. (1989). Maternal emotional signals, social referencing, and infants’ reactions to strangers. New Directions for Child Development, 44, 25–49.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Brown, W. (1960). Exploration in management. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
  3. Brown, W. (1962). Piecework abandoned: The effect of wage incentive systems on managerial authority. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
  4. Case, R. (1992). The role of the frontal lobes in the regulation of cognitive development. Brain and Cognition, 20, 51–73.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Commons, M. L. (2008a). Introduction to the model of hierarchical complexity and its relationship to postformal action. World Futures, 64, 305–320.Google Scholar
  6. Commons, M. L. (2008b). Selectionism and stage change: The dynamics of evolution, I. World Futures, 64, 348–360.Google Scholar
  7. Commons, M. L., & Bresette, L. M. (2006). Illuminating major creative scientific innovators with postformal stages. In C. Hoare (Ed.), Handbook of adult development and learning (pp. 255–280). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Commons, M. L., Bresette, L. M., & Ross, S. N. (2008). The connection between postformal thought and major scientific innovations. World Futures, 64, 503–512.Google Scholar
  9. Commons, M. L., & Goodheart, E. A. (2008). Cultural progress is the result of developmental level of support. World Futures, 64, 406–415.Google Scholar
  10. Commons, M. L., & Miller, P. M. (2002a). A complete theory of human evolution of intelligences must consider stage changes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Commons, M. L., & Miller, P. M. (2002b). A complete theory of empathy must consider stage changes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 30–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Commons, M. L., & Ross, S. N. (2008a). What postformal thought is, and why it matters. World Futures, 64, 321–329.Google Scholar
  13. Commons, M. L., & Ross, S. N. (2008b). Toward a cross-species measure of general intelligence. World Futures, 64, 383–398.Google Scholar
  14. Commons, M. L., & Ross, S. N. (2008c). The hierarchical complexity view of evolution and history. World Futures, 64, 399–405.Google Scholar
  15. Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype: The gene as the unit of selection. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
  17. Dawson, T. L. (2004). Assessing intellectual development: Three approaches, one sequence. Journal of Adult Development, 11, 71–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Day, J. M. (2008). Human development and the model of hierarchical complexity: Learning from research in the psychology of moral and religious development. World Futures, 64, 452–467.Google Scholar
  19. de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  20. de Waal, F. B. M., & Lanting, F. (1997). Bonobo: The forgotten ape. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  21. Field, T. (1989). Individual and maturational differences in infant expressivity. New Directions for Child Development, 44, 9–24.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T. R. (2006). Dynamic development of action and thought. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Theoretical models of human development, Vol. 1, pp. 313–399). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  24. Fowler, J. (1981). Stages of faith. San Francisco: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  25. Fowler, J. (1996). Faithful change. San Francisco: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  26. Griffin, S. (2005). Fostering the development of whole-number sense: Teaching mathematics in the primary grades. In M. S. Donovan & J. D. Bransford (Eds.), How students learn: History, mathematics and science in the classroom (pp. 257–308). Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.Google Scholar
  27. Hoffman, M. L. (1978). Toward a theory of empathic arousal and development. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The development of affect (pp. 227–256). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  28. Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence: An essay on the development of formal operational structures. (A. Parsons & S. Seagrim, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. (Originally published 1955).Google Scholar
  29. Jaques, E. (1976). A general theory of bureaucracy. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
  30. Jaques, E. (1996). Requisite organization: A total system for effective managerial organization and managerial leadership for the 21 st century. Arlington, VA: Cason Hall.Google Scholar
  31. Koplowitz, H. (2008). In praise of top-down decision making in managerial hierarchies. World Futures, 64, 513–523.Google Scholar
  32. Kuczynski, L., Kochanska, G., Radke-Yarrow, M., & Girnius-Brown, O. (1987). A developmental interpretation of young children’s noncompliance. Developmental Psychology, 23, 799–806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mascolo, M. F. (2008). The concept of domain in developmental analyses of hierarchical complexity. World Futures, 64, 330–347.Google Scholar
  34. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pepperberg, I. (1992). Proficient performance of a conjunction, recursive task by an African gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 295–305.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Piaget, J. (1971). The theory of stages in cognitive development. In D. R. Green, M. P. Ford, & G. B. Flamer (Eds.), Measurement and Piaget (pp. 1–11). New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
  37. Piaget, J. (1983). Piaget’s theory. In W. Kessen (Ed.), History, theory, and methods (pp. 103–126). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  38. Ross, S. N. (2008). Fractal transition steps to fractal stages: The dynamics of evolution, II. World Futures, 64, 361–374.Google Scholar
  39. Ross, S. N., & Commons, M. L. (2008). Applying hierarchical complexity to political development. World Futures, 64, 480–497.Google Scholar
  40. Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding: Developmental and clinical analyses. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  41. Wolfsont, C., Ross, S. N., Miller, P. M., Commons, M. L., & Chernoff, M. (2008). Domain-specific increases in stage of performance in a complete theory of the evolution of human intelligence. World Futures, 64, 416–429.Google Scholar
  42. Wynn, T. (1981). The intelligence of Oldowan hominids. Journal of Human Evolution, 10, 529–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Wynn, T. (1993). Two developments in the mind of early Homo. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 12, 299–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Young, G. (1990a). Early neuropsychological development: Lateralization of functions - hemispheric specialization. In C. A. Hauert (Ed.), Developmental psychology: Cognitive, perceptuo-motor and neuropsychological perspectives (pp. 113–181). Amsterdam: North Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Young, G. (1990b). The development of hemispheric and manual specialization. In G. E. Hammond (Ed.), Cerebral control of speech and limb movements (pp. 79–139). Amsterdam: North Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Young, G. (1997). Adult development, therapy, and culture: A postmodern synthesis. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  47. Zahn-Waxler, C., & Kochanska, G. (1988). The origins of guild. In R. A. Thompson (Ed.), Socioemotional development: Current theory and research in motivation (pp. 183–258). Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  48. Zahn-Waxler, C., Kochanska, G., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. A. (1979). Child rearing and children’s prosocial initiations toward victims of distress. Child Development, 50, 319–330.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Glendon CollegeYork UniversityTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations