Advertisement

The Use of Social Information in Chimpanzees and Dogs

  • Josep Call
Chapter
Part of the Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects book series (DIPR)

Abstract

When people are asked to rank animals with regard to their intelligence, dogs and chimpanzees are often cited as prime examples of smart animals. A couple of studies even show that not only students of an introductory psychology class but also professionals in animal learning and cognition rank dogs and chimpanzees as particularly intelligent animals (Eddy et al., 1993; Rasmussen et al., 1993). Intelligence, however, is a difficult concept to define because it has different meanings. Sometimes intelligence is used as a synonym of creativity and flexibility. Chimpanzees are clearly intelligent by this definition. They use tools, solve problems in various ways, and they can make inferences. Others use intelligence as equivalent to learning ability (Rumbaugh and Pate, 1984). This is extended to trainability in the case of dogs. Dogs clearly excel in this aspect as their many uses and training regimes show that dogs can learn various tasks with accuracy and they can be trained to cooperate with others.

Keywords

Social Information Capuchin Monkey Distant Space Head Direction Intentional Communication 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Agnetta, B., Hare, B., and Tomasello, M., 2000, Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canisfamiliaris) of different ages do and do not use, Anim. Cogn. 3: 107–112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, J. R., Sallaberry, P., and Barbier, H., 1995, Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by capuchin monkeys, Anim. Behav. 49: 201–208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Byrne, R W. and Whiten, A., 1988, Machiavellian Intelligence. Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans, Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  4. Call, J., 2001, Chimpanzee social cognition, Trends Cogn. Sci. 5: 369–405CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Call, J., 2003, Inferences about the location of food in the great apes, J. Comp. Psych. (in press).Google Scholar
  6. Call, J., Agnetta, B., and Tomasello, M., 2000, Social cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden objects, Anim. Cogn. 3: 23–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Call, J., Brauer, J., Kaminski, J., and Tomasello, M., 2003, Domestic dogs are sensitive to the attentional state of humans, J. Comp. Psych. 117: 257–263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Call, J., Hare, B. H., and Tomasello, M., 1998, Chimpanzee gaze following in an object-choice task, Anim. Cogn. 1: 89–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Call, J. and Tomasello, M., 1994, Production and comprehension of referential pointing by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), J. Comp. Psych. 108: 307–317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Call, J. and Tomasello, M., 1996, The effect of humans on the cognitive development of apes, in: Reaching into Thought, A. E. Russon, K. A. Bard, and S. T. Parker, eds., Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 371–403Google Scholar
  11. Call, J. and Tomasello, M., 1998, Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and human children (Homo sapiens), J. Comp. Psych. 112: 192–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. de Waal, F. B. M., 1988, The communicative repertoire of captive bonobos (Pan paniscus), compared to that of chimpanzees, Behaviour 106: 3–4Google Scholar
  13. de Waal, F. B. M., and van Hooff, J. A. R A. M., 1981, Side-directed communication and agonistic interactions in chimpanzees, Behaviour 77: 164–198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Eddy, T. J., Gallup G. G., Jr., and Povinelli, D. J., 1993, Attribution of cognitive states to animals: Anthropomorphism in comparative perspective, J. Soc. Issues 49: 87–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gomez, J. C, 1996, Non-human primate theories of (non-human primate) minds: Some issues concerning the origins of mind-reading, in: Theories of Theories of Mind, P. Carruthers and P. K. Smith, eds., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 330–343CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Goodall, J., 1986, The Chimpanzees ofGombe. Patterns of Behavior, Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  17. Gyger, M., Karakashian, S. J., and Marler, P., 1986, Avian alarm calling: Is there an audience effect? Anim. Behav. 34: 1570–1572CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C, and Tomasello, M., 2002, The domestication of social cognition in dogs, Science 298: 1634–1636CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., and Tomasello, M., 2000, Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see, Anim. Behav. 59: 771–785CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hare, B. H., Call, J., and Tomasello, M., 1998, Communication of food location between human and dog (Canisfamiliaris), Evol. Comm. 2: 137–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M., 2001, Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know and do not know? Anim. Behav. 61: 139–151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hare, B. and Tomasello, M., 1999, Domestic dogs (Canisfamiliaris) use human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food, J. Comp. Psych. 113: 173–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Herman, L. M., Abichandani, S. L., Elhajj, A. N., Herman, E. Y. K., Sanchez, J. L. et al., 1999, Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) comprehend the referential character of the human pointing gesture, J. Comp. Psych. 113: 347–364CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Itakura, S., 1996, An exploratory study of gaze-monitoring in nonhuman primates, Jpn. Psych. Res. 38: 174–180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Itakura, S., Agnetta, B., Hare, B., and Tomasello, M., 1999, Chimpanzees use human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food, Dev. Sci. 2: 448–456CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Itakura, S. and Anderson, J. R, 1996, Learning to use experimenter-given cues during an object-choice task by a capuchin monkey, Curr. Psych. Cogn. 15: 103–112Google Scholar
  27. Itakura, S., and Tanaka, M., 1998, Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and human infants (Homo sapiens), J. Comp. Psych. 112: 119–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kohler, W., 1925, The Mentality of Apes, Vintage Books, New York.Google Scholar
  29. Krause, M. A. and Fouts, R. S., 1997, Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) pointing: Hand shapes, accuracy, and the role of eye gaze, J. Comp. Psych. 111: 330–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Krebs, J. R and Dawkins, R, 1984, Animal signals: Mindreading and manipulation, in: Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, J. R Krebs, and N. B. Davies, eds., Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 380–402.Google Scholar
  31. Leavens, D. A. and Hopkins, W. D., 1998, Intentional communication by chimpanzees: A cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures, Dev. Psych. 34: 813–822CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D., and Bard, K. A., 1996, Indexical and referential pointing in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), J. Comp. Psych. 110: 346–353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lorenz, K., 1954, Man meets Dog, Penguin Books, New York.Google Scholar
  34. McKinley, J. and Sambrook, T. D., 2000, Use of human-given cues by domestic dogs (Canisfamiliaris) and horses (Equus caballus), Anim. Cogn. 3: 13–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Menzel, C. R, 1999, Unprompted recall and reporting of hidden objects by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) after extended delays, J. Comp. Psych. 113: 426–434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Menzel, E. W. Jr., 1974, A group of young chimpanzees in a one-acre field: Leadership and communication, in: Behavior of Nonhuman Primates, A. M. Schrier and F. Stollnitz, eds., Academic Press, New York, pp. 83–153.Google Scholar
  37. Miklosi, A, Polgardi, R. Topal, J. and Csanyi, V., 1998, Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs, Anim. Cogn. 1: 113–121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Miklosi, A.Polgardi, R.Topal, J.and Csanyi, V., 2000, Intentional behavior in dog-human communication: An experimental analysis of “showing” behaviour in the dog, Anim. Cogn. 3: 159–166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Miles, H. L. W., 1990, The cognitive foundations for reference in a signing orangutan, in: “Language” and Intelligence in Monkeys ana 1 Apes, S. T. Parker and K. R. Gibson, eds., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 511–539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Mitchell, R. W. and Thompson, N. S., 1986, Deception in play between dogs and people, in: Deception. ‘Perspectives on Human and Nonhuman Deceit, R. W. Mitchell and N. S. Thompson, eds., SUNY, New York, pp. 193–204.Google Scholar
  41. Patterson, R, 1978, Linguistic capabilities of a lowland gorilla, in: Sign Language and Language Acquisition in Man and Ape, F. C. C. Peng, ed., Westview Press, Boulder, CO, pp. 161–201.Google Scholar
  42. Povinelli, D. J. and Eddy, T. J., 1996a, What young chimpanzees know about seeing, Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 61 (3): 1–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Povinelli, D. J. and Eddy, T. J., 1996b, Chimpanzees: Joint visual attention, Psych. Sci. 7: 129–135CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Povinelli, D. J. and Eddy, T. J., 1997, Specificity of gaze-following in young chimpanzees, Br. J. Dev. Psych. 15: 213–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rasmussen, J. L., Rajecki, D. W., and Craft, H. D., 1993, Humans’ perceptions of animal mentality: Ascriptions of thinking, J. Comp. Psych. 107: 283–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Reaux, J. E., Theall, L. A., and Povinelli, D. J., 1999, A longitudinal investigation of chimpanzees’ understanding of visual perception, Child Dev. 70: 275–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Rumbaugh, D. M. and Pate, J. L., 1984, The evolution of cognition in primates: A comparative perspective, in: Animal Cognition, H. L. Roitblat, T. G. Bever, and H. S. Terrace, eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 569–587.Google Scholar
  48. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Mcdonald, K., Sevcik, R A., Hopkins, W. D., and Rubert, E., 1986, Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Panpaniscus),J. Exp. Psych.: Gen. 115: 211–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Scheumann, M. and Call, J., 2002, The use of experimenter-given cues by South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus), Anim. Cogn. (manuscript submitted for publication).Google Scholar
  50. Soproni, K., Miklosi, A., Topal, J., and Csanyi, V., 2001, Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canisfamiliaris), J. Comp. Psych. 115: 122–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Suddendorf, T. and Whiten, A., 2001, Mental evolution and development: Evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes and other animals, Psych. Bull. 127: 629–650CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Tomasello, M. and Call, J., 1997, Primate Cognition, Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  53. Tomasello, M., Call, J., and Gluckman, A., 1997a, Comprehension of novel communicative signs by apes and human children, Child Dev. 68: 1067–1080CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Tomasello, M., Call, J., and Hare, B. H., 1998, Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics, Anim. Behav. 55: 1063–1069CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Tomasello, M., Call, J., Nagell, K., Oiguin, R., and Carpenter, M., 1994, The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: A trans-generational study, Primates 35: 137–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Tomasello, M., Call, J., Warren, J., Frost, G. T., Carpenter, M., and Nagell, K., 1997b, The ontogeny of chimpanzee gestural signals: A comparison across groups and generations, Evol. Comm. 1: 223–259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Tomasello, M., George, B. L., Kruger, A. C, Farrar, M. J., and Evans, A., 1985, The development of gestural communication in young chimpanzees, J. Hum. Evol.14: 175–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Tomasello, M., Hare, B., and Agnetta, B., 1999, Chimpanzees follow gaze direction geometrically, Anim. Behav. 58: 769–777CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Tschudin, A., Call, J., Dunbar, R. I. M., Harris, G., and van der Elst, C, 2001, Comprehension of signs by dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), J. Comp. Psych. 115: 100–105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Vick, S. J. and Anderson, J. R, 2000, Learning and limits of use of eye gaze by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) in an object-choice task, J. Comp. Psych. 114: 200–207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Whiten, A. and Byrne, R W., 1997, Machiavellian Intelligence II. Extensions and Evaluations, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Josep Call
    • 1
  1. 1.Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany

Personalised recommendations