Skip to main content

Dynamical Systems and Convergence

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 553 Accesses

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Cognitive and Neural Systems ((SSCNS,volume 1))

Abstract

Dynamical systems can be coupled to other dynamical systems to cover organismic phenomenon on many scales, as they provides a general frame applicable to an ample range of phenomena. Describing a system as coupled systems highlights interfaces. Interfaces reflect the locus of organized transitions, or level crossings, places where constancy and variability merge. At interfaces there is convergence and/or divergence, so the vicissitudes of variation coalesce in meaningful averages; whereas conversely, constancy may drift and spread into variation. These transformations depend crucially on a number of processes, which are regimented by particular types of physical interactions. Variability may coalesce in averages, whereas constancy may drift. Mutatis mutandis, the same operating principles – large numbers and physical laws – means smear again in variation. In a functioning system, these two stances of constancy and variation complement and define each other.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Causes in a model are abstracted from the phenomenon in a particular level of analysis, and may not reflect the exact interactions of matter, but interesting slices of the phenomenon.

  2. 2.

    This is not inherently the case, for instance, in information theory approaches, nor is it for symbolic computation (good old-fashioned artificial intelligence) approaches.

  3. 3.

    One word about the role of parameters in a dynamical systems model. An often-voiced criticism has been that “given enough parameters, a model dances and sings (does anything).” Or “give me enough variables and I will create a green elephant”; the humorous comment is only appropriate on occasion, most often when directed towards models fitting curves. But in the case of dynamical systems, it is misapplied, a gross canard. For parameters in dynamical systems are very informative indeed. For one thing, they have the power of talking back to the phenomenon, contingent on the level of detail of the model. When a set of parameters that causes a certain model to behave as expected is found, it also says more about the phenomenon itself. Furthermore, qualitative analysis of dynamics based on parameter sets, as we will see in Sect. ??, informs about a wide breadth of dynamical behavior.

  4. 4.

    Abduction is the process of selection of sorts (state variables, parameters, rules) of a phenomenon to construct a model.

  5. 5.

    We know of other contributions to the muscle forces, such as bodily factors (e.g., dopamine controls muscle tonicity), whose influence may be modeled as modulatory. These factors can be modeled as parameters which may interface levels. For example, dopamine modulation of the force on a muscle may be modeled as a scaling factor of muscle-cell contraction.

  6. 6.

    This level is not satisfactory for longer intervals, for the forces on the muscles are changing nonlinearly with a number of extra factors, such as neurotransmitters, and energy sources to the contracting cells.

  7. 7.

    The experiments to come will draw heavily on the idea of coupled dynamics, and therefore I went to lengths to devise how to cover a natural system with coupled dynamical systems. The description was purely conceptual, but it is essentially the same kind of abstraction we employ when building models of behavior. Conclusions about the role and the forms of exchanges at interfaces is where much headway can be made in understanding the interplay between constancy and variability.

  8. 8.

    Detail about the modeled state variables and rules can be added, of course, ad libitum. But the amount of detail introduced should be governed by knowledge gain. If our level of abstraction is that of the overall contraction force in a small time window, other variables can be excluded, such as the production and release of neurotransmitters, or neuromodulators such as noradrenaline (epinephrine). We abstract away neuromodulators, synaptic alterations (facilitation, depression), and production of neurotransmitters to better comprehend how motor commands are conveyed. The explanations of motor behavior, even after this blunt abstraction, are not impaired but strengthened, as they introduce further constraints for equivalence classes of neural behavior.

References

  1. Claude G et al (2000) Principles of Neural Science, chaps. 33, 34, 35, 36, 37 (Part 6: Movement), 4th edn. Appleton & Lange, East Norwalk, CT

    Google Scholar 

  2. Arbib MA (1972) The metaphorical brain, an introduction to cybernetics and brain theory. MIT, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  3. Beer RD (1995) A dynamical systems perspective on agent-environment interaction. Artif Intell (72):173–215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Beer RD (2000) Dynamical approaches to cognitive science. Trends Cognit Sci 4(3):91–99

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Cruse H, Kindermann T, Schumm M, Dean J, Schmitz J (1998) Walknet–a biologically inspired network to control six-legged walking. Neural Netw 11(7–8):1435–1447

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Elman J (1990) Finding structure in time. Cogn Sci 14(2):179–211

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Freeman WJ (1995) Society of Brains: A study in the neuroscience of love and hate. Laurence Erlbaum Associates Inc, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  8. van Gelder T (1998) The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science. Behav Brain Sci 21(05):615–628

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Hofstadter D (1999) Gödel, Escher, Bach: An eternal golden braid. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  10. Jonas H (2001) The phenomenon of life. Northwestern University Press, Evanston, IL (1966)

    Google Scholar 

  11. Kelso J (1995) Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. MIT, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  12. Merleau-Ponty M (1942) The structure of behavior (1963 translation). Duquesne University Press, Philadelphia, PA

    Google Scholar 

  13. Newell A (1981) The knowledge level. Department of Computer Science, Carnegie-Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA

    Google Scholar 

  14. Schoner G, Kelso J (1988) Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. Science 239(4847):1513–1520

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Smith L, Thelen E (1993) A dynamic systems approach to development: Applications. MIT, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  16. Taylor C (1999) The atomists leucippus and democritus: Fragments: A text and translation. University of Toronto Press, Toronto, ON

    Google Scholar 

  17. von Uexküll J (1934) Bedeutungslehre/Streifzüge durch die Umwelten von Tieren und Menschen, 1956th edn. Rowohlt, Hamburg

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mario Negrello .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Negrello, M. (2011). Dynamical Systems and Convergence. In: Invariants of Behavior. Springer Series in Cognitive and Neural Systems, vol 1. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-8804-1_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics