Skip to main content

Physiological and Psychological Foundations of Sensory Function

  • Chapter
Sensory Evaluation of Food

Part of the book series: Food science text series ((FSTS))

Abstract

The oldest branch of experimental psychology is that of psychophysics, the study of relationships between physical stimuli and sensory experience. The first true psychophysical theorist was the 19th-century German physiologist, E. H. Weber. Building on earlier observations by Bernoulli and others, Weber noted that the amount a physical stimulus needed to be increased to be just perceivably different was a constant ratio. Thus, 14.5 ounces and 15 ounces could be told apart with great difficulty, and the same could be said of 29 and 30 ounces or 14.5 and 15 drams (Boring, 1942).

There is no conception in man’s mind which hath not at first, totally or in parts, been begotten upon by the organs of sense. —Thomas Hobbes, Leviathan, 1651.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Amoore, J.E. 1971. Olfactory genetics and anosmia. In L.M. Beidler, ed. Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Springer, Berlin, pp. 245–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amoore, J.E. and Forrester, L.J. 1976. The specific anosmia to trimethylamine: the fishy primary odor. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 2, 49–56.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Amoore, J.E., Forrester, L.J., and Pelosi, P. 1976. Specific anosmia to isobutyraldehyde: the malty primary odor. Chemical Senses, 2, 17–25.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Amoore, J.E., Venstrom, D., and Davis, A.R. 1968. Measurement of specific anosmia. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 26, 143–164.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, R.K., Lund, J.P., and Puil, E. 1978. Enkephalin and substance P effects related to trigeminal pain. Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 56, 216–222.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, N. 1974. Algebraic models in perception. In E.C. Carterette and M.P. Friedman, eds. Handb000k of Perception. IL Psychophysical Judgment and Measurement. Academic, New York, pp. 215–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arvidson, K. 1979. Location and variation in number of taste buds in human fungi-form papillae. Scandinavian Journal of Dental Research, 87, 435–442.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • ASTM. 1989. Standard definitions of terms relating to sensory evaluation of materials and products. In Annual Book of ASTM Standards. American Society for Testing and Materials, Philadelphia, p. 2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ayya, N., and Lawless, H.T. 1992. Qualitative and quantitative evaluation of high-in- tensity sweeteners and sweetener mixtures. Chemical Senses, 17, 245–259.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bartoshuk, L.M. 1978. History of taste reasearch. In E.C. Carterette and M.P. Friedman, eds. Handbook of Perception. IVI, Tasting and Smelling. Academic, New York, pp. 2–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartoshuk, L.M. 1979. Bitter taste of saccharin related to the genetic ability to taste the bitter substance 6-N-Propylthiouracil. Science, 205, 934–935.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bartoshuk, L.M., Cain, W.S., Cleveland, C.T., Grossman, L.S., Marks, L.E., Stevens, J.C., and Stolwijk, J.A. 1974. Saltiness of monosodium glutamate and sodium intake. Journal of the American Medical Association, 230, 670.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bartoshuk, L.M., Duffy, V.B., and Miller, I.J. 1994. PTC/PROP tasting: anatomy, psychophysics and sex effects. Physiology and Behavior, 56, 1165–1171.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bartoshuk, L.M., Murphy, C.L., and Cleveland, C.T. 1978. Sweet taste of dilute NaCl. Physiology and Behavior, 21, 609–613.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bate Smith, E.C. 1954. Astrigency in foods. Food Processing and Packaging, 23, 124–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birnbaum, M.H. 1982. Problems with so called “direct” scaling. In J.T. Kuznicki, A.F. Rutkiewic, and R.A. Johnson, eds. Problems and Approaches to Measuring Hedonics (ASTM STP 773 ). American Society for Testing and Materials, Philadelphia, pp. 34–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blakeslee, A.F. 1932. Genetics of sensory thresholds: taste for phenylthiocarbamide. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 18, 120–130.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E.G. 1942. Sensation and Perception in the History of Experimental Psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brennand, C.P., Ha, J.K., and Lindsay, R.C. 1989. Aroma properties and thresholds of some branched-chain and other minor volatile fatty acids occurring in milk-fat and meat lipids. Journal of Sensory Studies, 4, 105–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brud, W.S. 1986. Words versus odors: how perfumers communicate. Perfumer and Flavorist, 11, 27–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, L., and Axel, R. 1991. A novel multigene family may encode odorant receptors: a molecular basis for odor recognition. Cell, 65, 175–187.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Burgard, D.R., and Kuznicki, J.T. 1990. Chemometrics: Chemical and Sensory Data. CRC, Boca Raton, FL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burks, T.F., Buck, S.H., and Miller, M.S. 1985. Mechanisms of depletion of substance P by capsaicin. Federation Proceedings, 44, 2531–2534.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buttery, R.G., Teranishi, R., Flath, R.A., and Ling, L.C. 1989. Fresh tomato volatiles. In R.G.B. and R. Teranishi, eds. Flavor Chemistry. American Chemical Society, Washington, DC, p. 213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cagan, R.H. 1981. Recognition of taste stimuli at the initial binding interaction. In R.H. Cagan and M.R. Kare, eds. Biochemistry of Taste and Olfaction. Academic, New York, pp. 175–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, W.S. 1977. Differential sensitivity for smell: “noise” at the nose. Science, 195 (25 Feb.), 796–798.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, W.S. 1979. To know with the nose: keys to odor identification. Science, 203, 467–470.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, W.S. and Drexler, M. 1974. Scope and evaluation of odor counteraction and masking. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 237, 427–439.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, W.S. and Engen, T. 1969. Olfactory adaptation and the scaling of odor intensity. In C. Pfaffmann, ed. Olfaction and Taste III. Rockefeller University Press, New York, pp. 127–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, W.S. and Murphy, C.L. 1980. Interaction between chemoreceptive modalities of odor and irritation. Nature, 284, 255–257.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carterette, E.C., and Friedman, M.P., eds. 1978. Handb000k of Perception. II. Psychophysical Judgment and Measurement. Academic, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caul, J.F. 1957. The profile method of flavor analysis. Advances in Food Research, 7, 1–40.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chastrette, M., Elmouaffek, E., and Sauvegrain, P. 1988. A multidimensional statistical study of similarities between 74 notes used in perfumery. Chemical Senses, 13, 295–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Civille, G.L., and Lawless, H.T. 1986. The importance of language in describing perceptions. Journal of Sensory Studies, 1, 203–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Claassen, M., and Lawless, H.T. 1992. Comparison of descriptive terminology systems for sensory evaluation of fluid milk. Journal of Food Science, 57, 596–600, 621.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cliff, M., and Heymann, H. 1992. Descriptive analysis of oral pungency. Journal of Sensory Studies, 7, 279–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clifford, M.N. 1986. Phenol-protein interactions and their possible significance for astringency. In G.G. Birch and M.G. Lindley eds. Interactions of Food Components. Elsevier, London, pp. 143–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collings, V.B. 1974. Human taste response as a function of locus on the tongue and soft palate. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 169–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cometto-Mufliz, J.E. and Cain, W.S. 1984. Temporal integration of pungency. Chemical Senses, 8, 315–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cometto-Mufliz, J.E. and Hernandez, S.M. 1990. Odorous and pungent attributes of mixed and unmixed odorants. Perception and Psychophysics, 47, 391–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowart, B.J. 1987. Oral chemical irritation: does it reduce perceived taste intensity? Chemical Senses, 12, 467–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, D.W., Stevens, D.A., and Lawless, H.T. 1984. Perceived intensity of the taste of sugar mixtures and acid mixtures. Chemical Senses, 9, 107–120.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Desor, J.A., and Beauchamp, G.K. 1974. The human capacity to transmit olfactory information. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 551–556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamant, H., Oakley, B., Strom, L., and Zotterman, Y. 1965. A comparison of neural and psychophysical responses to taste stimuli in man. Acta Physiological Scandinavica, 64, 67–74.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Doty, R.L. 1991. Psychophysical measurement of odor perception in humans. In D.G. Laing, R.L. Doty, and W. Breipohl, eds. The Human Sense of Smell. Springer, Berlin, pp. 95–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doty, R.L., Brugger, W.E., Jurs, P.C., Orndorff, M.A., Snyder, P.J., and Lowry, L.D. 1978. Intranasal trigeminal stimulation from odorous volatiles: psychometric responses from anosmic and normal humans. Physiology and Behavior, 20, 175–185.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dravnieks, A. 1982. Odor quality: semantically generated multidimensional profiles are stable. Science, 218, 799–801.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Engen, T. 1982. The perception of odors. Academic, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engen, T. and Pfaffmann, C. 1959. Absolute judgments of odor intensity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 23–26.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Engen, T., and Ross, B. 1974. Long term memory for odors. Journal of Experimental Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farbman, A.I., and Hellekant, G. 1978. Quantitative analyses of fiber population in rat chords tympani nerves and fungiform papillae. American Journal of Anatomy, 153, 509–521.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fechner, G.T. 1966 (translation, orig. 1860). In D.H. Howes and E.G. Boring, eds. Elements of Psychophysics. E.H. Adler trans. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finger, T.E., and Silver W.L., eds. 1987. Neurobiology of Taste and Smell. Wiley Interscience, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, A.L. 1932. The relationship between chemical constitution and taste. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 18, 115–120.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R.A., and Byram, J. 1988. Taste-smell interactions are tastant and odorant dependent. Chemical Senses, 13, 445.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R.A., Ducheny, K., and Mize, S.J.S. 1989a. Strawberry odor but not red color enhances the sweetness of sucrose solutions. Chemical Senses, 14, 371.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R.A., Mize, S.J., and Carter, R. 1989b. An assessment of binary mixture interactions for nine sweeteners. Chemical Senses, 14, 621–632.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R.A., Wessel, N., and Shaffer, G. 1990. The enhancement of sweetness by strawberry odor is instruction dependent. Chemical Senses, 15, 576–577.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geldard, F.A. 1972. The Human Senses. 2d ed. Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Getchell, T.V., Doty, R.L., Bartoshuk, L.M., and Snow, J.B., eds. 1991. Smell and Taste in Health and Disease. Raven, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, B.G. 1985. Menthol modulates oral sensations of warmth and cold. Physiology and Behavior, 35, 427–434.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Green, B.G. 1986. Menthol inhibits the perception of warmth. Physiology and Behavior, 38, 833–838.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Green, B.G. 1989. Capsaicin sensitization and desensitization on the tongue produced by brief exposures to a low concentration. Neuroscience Letters, 107, 173–178.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Green, B.G. and Gelhard, B. 1989. Salt as an oral irritant. Chemical Senses, 14, 259–271.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Green, B.G. and Lawless, H.T. 1991. The psychophysics of somatosensory chemoreception in the nose and mouth. In Smell and Taste in Health and Disease. T. V. Getchell, R.L. Doty, L.M. Bartoshuk, and J.B. Snow, eds., Raven, New York, pp. 235–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, C.A. 1991. Structural organization of the olfactory system. In T.V. Getchell, R.L. Doty, L.M. Bartoshuk, and J.B. Snow, eds. Smell and Taste in Health and Disease. Raven, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grushka, M., and Sessle, B.J. 1991. Burning mouth syndrome. In T.V. Getchell, R.L. Doty, L.M. Bartoshuk, and J.B. Snow, eds. Smell and Taste in Health and Disease. Raven, New York, pp. 665–682.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guinard, J.-X., Pangborn, R.M., and Lewis, M.J. 1986. The time course of astringency in wine on repeated ingestion. American Journal of Enology and Viticulture, 37, 184–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gwartney, E., and Heymann, H. 1995. The temporal perception of menthol. Journal of Sensory Studies, 10, 393–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gwartney, E., and Heymann, H. 1996. Profiling to describe the sensory characteristics of a simple model menthol solution. Journal of Sensory Studies, 11, 39–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, M.L., Bartoshuk, L.M., Cain, W.S., and Stevens, J.C. 1975. PTC taste blindness and the taste of caffeine. Nature, 253, 442–443.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hernandez, S. 1997. A method of adjustment for solid foods and ethnic food preferences. M.S. thesis, Cornell University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobbes, T. 1651. Leviathan: or the Matter, Forme and Power of a Commonwealth Ecclesiastical and Civil. M. Oakeshott, ed. Collier Books, New York, 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jansco, N. 1960. Role of the nerve terminals in the mechanism of inflammatory reactions. Bulletin of Millard Fillmore Hospital, Buffalo, 7, 53–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karrer, T., and Bartoshuk, L.M. 1995. Effects of capsaicin desensitization on taste in humans. Physiology and Behavior, 57, 421–429.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kauer, J.S. 1987. Coding in the Olfactory System. In T.E. Finger and W.L. Silver, eds. Neurobiology of Taste and Smell. Wiley, New York, pp. 205–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawamura, Y., and Kare, M.R. 1987. Umami: A Basic Taste. Dekker, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroeze, J.H.A. 1979. Masking and adaptation of sugar sweetness intensity. Physiology and Behavior, 22, 347–351.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kroeze, J.H.A. and Bartoshuk, L.M. 1985. Bitterness suppression as revealed by split-tongue taste stimulation in humans. Physiology and Behavior, 35, 779–783.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Laing, D.G., Livermore, B.A., and Francis, G.W. 1991. The human sense of smell has a limited capacity for identifying odors in mixtures. Chemical Senses, 16, 392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laing, D.G., and Willcox, M.E. 1983. Perception of components in binary odor mixtures. Chemical Senses, 7, 249–264.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Laska, M., and Hudson, R. 1992. Ability to discriminate between related odor mixtures. Chemical Senses, 17, 403–415.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. 1977. The pleasantness of mixtures in taste and olfaction. Sensory Processes, 1, 227–237.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. 1979a. Evidence for neural inhibition in bittersweet taste mixtures. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 93, 538–547.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. 1979b. The taste of creatine and creatinine. Chemical Senses, 4, 249–252.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. 1980. A comparison of different methods for assessing sensitivity to the taste of phenylthiocarbamide PTC. Chemical Senses, 5, 247–256.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. 1984. Oral chemical irritation: psychophysical properties. Chemical Senses, 9, 143–155.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. 1987. An olfactory analogy to release from mixture suppression in taste. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 25, 266–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. 1996. Flavor. In E.C. Carterrette and M.P. Friedman, eds. Cognitive Ecology. Academic, San Diego, pp. 325–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Clark, C.C. 1992. Psychological biases in time intensity scaling. Food Technology, 46(11), 81, 84–86, 90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Corrigan, C.J. 1994. Semantics of astringency. In Olfaction and Taste XI. Proceedings of the 11th International Symposium on Olfaction and Taste, and 27th Meeting, Japanese Association for Smell and Taste Sciences. K. Kurihara, ed. Springer, Tokyo, pp. 288–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Engen, T. 1977. Associations to odors: interference, mnemonics and verbal labeling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 52–57.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Gillette, M. 1985. Sensory Responses to Oral Chemical Heat. In D.D. Bills and C.J. Mussinan, eds. Characterization and Measurement of Flavor Compounds. American Chemical Society, Washington, DC, pp. 27–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Malone, G.J. 1986. Comparisons of rating scales: sensitivity, replicates and relative measurement. Journal of Sensory Studies, 1, 155–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Schlegel, M.P. 1984. Direct and indirect scaling of taste-odor mixtures. Journal of Food Science, 49, 44–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Stevens, D.A. 1983. Cross-adaptation of sucrose and intensive sweeteners. Chemical Senses, 7, 309–315.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Stevens, D.A. 1984. Effects of oral chemical irritation on taste. Physiology and Behavior, 32, 995–998.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Stevens, D.A. 1988. Responses by humans to oral chemical irritants as a function of locus of stimulation. Perception and Psychophysics, 43, 72–78.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Stevens, D.A. 1989. Mixtures of oral chemical irritants. In D.G. Laing, W.S. Cain, R.L. McBride and B.W. Ache, eds. Perception of Complex Smells and Tastes. Academic, Sydney, Australia, pp. 297–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Stevens, D.A. 1990. Differences between and interactions of oral irritants: neuro-physiological and perceptual implications. In B.G. Green and J.R. Mason, eds. Chemical Irritation in the Nose and Mouth. Dekker, New York, pp. 197–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T. and Zwillenberg, D. 1983. Clinical methods for testing taste and olfaction. Transactions of the Pennsylvania Academy of Ophthalmology and Otolaryngology, Fall 1983, 190–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T., Horne, J., and Giasi, P. 1996. Astringency of acids is related to pH. Chemical Senses, 21, 397–403.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T., Rozin, P., and Shenker, J. 1985. Effects of oral capsaicin on gustatory, olfactory and irritant sensations and flavor identification in humans who regularly or rarely consume chili pepper. Chemical Senses, 10, 579–589.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T., Thomas, C.J.C., and Johnston, M. 1995. Variation in odor thresholds for 1-carvone and cineole and correlations with suprathreshold intensity ratings. Chemical Senses, 20, 9–17.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H.T., Antinone, M.J., Ledford, R.A., and Johnston, M. 1994. Olfactory responsiveness to diacetyl. Journal of Sensory Studies, 9, 47–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, C.B., and Lawless, H.T. 1991. Time-course of astringent sensations. Chemical Senses, 16, 225–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehninger, A.L. 1975. Biochemistry. 2d ed. Worth, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marin, A.B., Acree, T.E., and Hotchkiss, J. 1987. Effects of orange juice packaging on the aroma of orange juice. Paper presented at the 194th ACS National Meeting, New Orleans, LA, Sep. 1987.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marin, A.B., Barnard, J., Darlington, R.B. and Acree, T.E. 1991. Sensory thresholds: estima- tion from dose-response curves. Journal of Sensory Studies, 6 (4), 205–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattes, R., and Labov, J. 1989. Bitter taste responses to phenylthiocarbamide are not related to dietary goitrogen intake in human beings. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 89 (5), 692–694.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mattes, R., and Lawless, H.T. 1985. An adjustment error in optimization of taste intensity. Appetite, 6, 103–114.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McBride, R.L. 1983. A JND-scale/category scale convergence in taste. Perception and Psychophysics, 34, 77–83.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McBride, R.L. 1987. Taste psychophysics and the Beilder equation. Chemical Senses, 12, 323–332.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McBurney, D.H. 1966. Magnitude estimation of the taste of sodium chloride after adaptation to sodium chloride. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 869–873.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBurney, D.H. and Bartoshuk, L.M. 1973. Interactions between stimuli with different taste qualities. Physiology and Behavior, 10, 1101–1106.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McBurney, D.H. and Shick, T.R. 1971. Taste and water taste of 26 compounds for man. Perception and Psychophysics, 11, 228–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBurney, D.H., Smith, D.V., and Shick, T.R. 1972. Gustatory cross-adaptation, sourness and bitterness. Perception and Psychophysics, 11, 228–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • McManus, J.P., Davis, K.G., Lilley, T.H., and Halsam, E. 1981. Polyphenol interac- tions. Journal of the Chemical Society, Chemical Communications, 309–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meilgaard, M.C., Reid, D.S., and Wyborski, K.A. 1982. Reference standards for beer flavor terminology system. Journal of the American Society of Brewing Chemists, 40, 119–128.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meiselman, H.L. 1971. Effect of presentation procedure on taste intensity functions. Perception and Psychophysics, 10, 15–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meiselman, H.L. and Halpern, B.P. 1973. Enhancement of taste intensity through pulsatile stimulation. Physiology and Behavior, 11, 713–716.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, I.J., and Bartoshuk, L.M. 1991. Taste perception, taste bud distribution and spatial relationships. In T.V. Getchell, R.L. Doty, L.M. Bartoshuk, and J.B. Snow, eds. Smell and Taste in Health and Disease. Raven, New York, pp. 205–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, I.J., and Spangler, K.M. 1982. Taste bud distribution and innervation on the palate of the rat. Chemical Senses, 7, 99–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moio, L., Langlois, D., Etievant, P.X., and Addeo, F. 1993. Powerful odorants in water buffalo and bovine mozzarella cheese by use of extract dilution sniffing analysis. Italian Journal of Food Science, 3, 227–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz, H.R. 1971. The sweetness and pleasantness of sugars. American Journal of Psychology, 84, 387–405.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz, H.R. 1983. Product Testing and Sensory Evaluation of Foods. Food and Nutrition Press. Westport, CT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mozell, M.M. 1970. Evidence for a chromatographic model of olfaction. Journal of General Physiology, 56, 25–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, N.J., Williamson, M.P., Lilley, T.H., and Haslam, E. 1994. Study of the interaction between salivary proline-rich proteins and a polyphenol by ‘H-NMR spectroscopy. European Journal of Biochemistry, 219, 923–935.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, C. and Cain, W.S. 1980. Taste and olfaction: independence vs. interaction. Physiology and Behavior, 24, 601–605.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, C., Cain, W.S., and Bartoshuk, L.M. 1977. Mutual action of taste and olfaction. Sensory Processes, 1, 204–211.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nagodawithana, T.W. 1995. Savory Flavors. Esteekay Associates, Milwaukee, WI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nejad, M.S. 1986. The neural activities of the greater superficial petrosal nerve of the rat in response to chemical stimulation of the palate. Chemical Senses, 11, 283–293.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, A.C., Arnold, R.A., Buechsenstein, J., Leach, E.J., Schmidt, J.O., and Stern, P.M. 1987. Modification of a standardized system of wine aroma terminology. American Journal of Enology and Viticulture, 38 (2), 143–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Mahony, M., and Ishii, R. 1986. Umami taste concept: implications for the dogma of four basic tastes. In Y. Kawamura and M.R. Kare, eds. Umami: A Basic Taste. Dekker, New York, pp. 75–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pangborn, R.M. 1988. Relationship of personal traits and attitudes to acceptance of food attributes. In J. So1ms, D.A. Booth, P.M. Pangborn, and O. Rainhardt, eds. Food Acceptance and Nutrition. Academic, New York, pp. 353–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pangborn, R.M. and Braddock, K.S. 1989. Ad libitum preferences for salt in chicken broth. Food Quality and Preference, 1, 47–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pangborn, R.M. and Dunkley, W.L. 1964. Laboratory procedures for evaluating the sensory properties of milk. Dairy Science Abstracts, 26, 55–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelosi, P., and Pisanelli, A.M. 1981. Specific anosmia to 1,8-cineole: the camphor primary odor. Chemical Senses, 6, 87–93.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pelosi, P., and Viti, R. 1978. Specific anosima to I-carvone: the minty primary odour. Chemical Senses and Flavour, 3, 331–337.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Perng, C.M., and McDaniel, M.R. 1989. Optimization of a blackberry juice drink using response surface methodology. In Institute of Food Technologists, Program and Abstracts, Annual Meeting. Institute of Food Technologists, Chicago, p. 216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philips, L.G., McGiff, M.L., Barbano, D.M., and Lawless, H.T. 1995. The influence of nonfat dry milk on the sensory properties, viscosity and color of low-fat milks Journal of Dairy Science, 78, 2113–2118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prescott, J., and Stevenson, R.J. 1995. Effects of oral chemical irritation on tastes and flavors in frequent and infrequent users of chili. Physiology and Behavior, 58, 1117–1127.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Prescott, J., and Stevenson, R.J. 1996. Psychophysical responses to single and multiple presentations of the oral irritant zingerone: relationship to frequency of chili consumption. Physiology and Behavior, 60, 617–624.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Prescott, J., Allen, S., and Stevenson, R.J. 1993. Interactions between oral chemical irritation, taste and temperature. Chemical Senses, 18, 389–404.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P. 1982. “Taste-smell confusions” and the duality of the olfactory sense. Perception and Psychophysics, 31, 397–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubico, S.M., and McDaniel, M.R. 1992. Sensory evaluation of acids by free-choice profiling. Chemical Senses, 17, 273–289.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schifferstein, H.N.J., and Fritjers, J.E.R. 1991. The perception of the taste of KC1, NaC1 and quinine HC1 is not related to PROP sensitivity, Chemical Senses 16 (4), 303–317.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, T.R., and Plata-Salaman, C.R. 1991. Coding of taste quality. In T.V. Getchell, R.L. Doty, L.M. Bartoshuk, and J.B. Snow, eds. Smell and Taste in Health and Disease. Raven, New York, pp. 345–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sizer, F., and Harris, N. 1985. The influence of common food additives and temperature on threshold perception of capsaicin. Chemical Senses, 10, 279–286.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, D.A., and Lawless, H.T. 1986. Putting out the fire: effects of tastants on oral chemical irritation. Perception and Psychophysics, 39, 346–350.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, D.A., and Lawless, H.T. 1987 Enhancement of responses to sequential presentation of oral chemical irritants. Physiology and Behavior, 39, 63–65.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, J.C., Cain, W.S., and Burke, R.J. 1988. Variability of olfactory thresholds. Chemical Senses, 13, 643–653.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S.S. 1957. On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64, 153–181.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S.S. 1959. Cross-modality validation of subjective scales for loudness, vibration and electric shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 201–209.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S.S. 1962. The surprising simplicity of sensory metrics. American Psychologist, 17, 29–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S.S. and Galanter, E.H. 1957. Ratio scales and category scales for a dozen perceptual continua. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 377–411.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R.J., Prescott, J., and Boakes, R.A. 1995. The acquisition of taste properties by odors. Learning and Motivation 26, 433–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stillman, J.A. 1993. Color influence flavor identification in fruit-flavored beverages. Journal of Food Science, 58, 810–812.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stryer, L. 1995. Biochemistry, 4th ed. Freeman, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szolscanyi, J. 1977. A pharmacological approach to elucidation of the role of different nerve fibers and receptor endings in mediation of pain. Journal of Physiology (Paris), 73, 251–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, C.J.C., and Lawless, H.T. 1995. Astringent subqualities in acids. Chemical Senses, 20, 593–600.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, D. 1971. Nonolfactory responses from the nasal cavity: Jacobson’s Organ and the trigeminal system. In L.M. Beidler, eds. Handbook of Sensory Physiology IV(I). Springer, Berlin, pp. 151–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuorila, H. 1986. Sensory profiles of milks with varying fat contents. Lebensmittel Wissenschaft and Technologie, 19, 344–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Klaauw, N.J., and Frank, R.A. 1996. Scaling component intensities of complex stimuli: the influence of response alternatives. Environment International, 22, 21–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Sydow, E., Moskowitz, H., Jacobs, H., and Meiselman, H. 1974. Odor-taste interactions in fruit juices. Lebensmittel Wissenschaft and Technologie, 7, 18–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiffenbach, J.M. 1991. Chemical Senses in Aging. In T.V. Getchell, R.L. Doty, L.M. Bartoshuk, and J.B. Snow, eds. Smell and Taste in Health and Disease. New York, pp. 369–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, M.C., Beeman, C.S., and Kinsella, B.A. 1985. Distribution of taste and general sensory nerve endings in fungiform papillae of the hamster. American Journal of Anatomy, 173, 185–201.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wiseman, J.J., and McDaniel, M.R. 1989. Modification of fruit flavors by aspartame and sucrose. In Institute of Food Technologists, Chicago: Annual Meeting Abstracts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers, Z. 1991. Sound perception and food quality. Journal of Food Quality, 14, 87–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wysocki, C., and Beauchamp, G.K. 1988. Ability to smell androstenone is genetically determined Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 81, 4899–4902.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, S. 1967. The synergistic taste effect of monosodium glutamate and disodium 5’inosinate. Journal of Food Science, 32, 473–475.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, S. and Kobori, I. 1994. Humans and appreciation of umami taste. Olfaction and Taste XI. Proceedings of the 11th International Symposium on Olfaction and Taste and 27th Meeting, Japanese Association for Smell and Taste Sciences. In K. Kurihara, ed. Springer, Tokyo, pp. 353–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yau, N.J.N., and McDaniel, M.R. 1990. The power function of carbonation. Journal of Sensory Studies, 5, 117–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yau, N.J.N., and McDaniel, M.R. 1991. The effect of temperature on carbonation perception. Chemical Senses, 16, 337–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yokomukai, Y., Cowart, B.J. and Beauchamp, G.K. 1993. Individual differences in sensitivity to bitter-tasting substances. Chemical Senses, 18, 669–681.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yost, W.A., and Nielsen, D.W. 1977. Fundamentals of Hearing. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1999 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lawless, H.T., Heymann, H. (1999). Physiological and Psychological Foundations of Sensory Function. In: Sensory Evaluation of Food. Food science text series. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-7452-5_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-7452-5_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4757-6499-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4419-7452-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics