Skip to main content

Consciousness, Decision-Making and Neural Computation

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Perception-Action Cycle

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Cognitive and Neural Systems ((SSCNS))

Abstract

Computational processes that are closely related to conscious processing and reasoning are described. Evidence is reviewed that there are two routes to action, the explicit, conscious, one involving reasoning, and an implicit, unconscious route for well-learned actions to obtain goals. Then a higher order syntactic thought (HOST) computational theory of consciousness is described. It is argued that the adaptive value of higher order syntactic thoughts is to solve the credit assignment problem that arises if a multi-step syntactic plan needs to be corrected. It is then suggested that it feels like something to be an organism that can think about its own linguistic and semantically based thoughts. It is suggested that qualia, raw sensory and emotional feels, arise secondarily to having evolved such a HOST processing system, and that sensory and emotional processing feels like something because it would be unparsimonious for it to enter the planning, HOST system and not feel like something. Neurally plausible models of decision-making are described, which are based on noise-driven and therefore probabilistic integrate-and-fire attractor neural networks, and it is proposed that networks of this type are involved when decisions are made between the explicit and implicit routes to action. This raises interesting issues about free will. It has been argued that the confidence one has in one’s decisions provides an objective measure of awareness, but it is shown that two coupled attractor networks can account for decisions based on confidence estimates from previous decisions. In analyses of the implementation of consciousness, it is shown that the threshold for access to the consciousness system is higher than that for producing behavioural responses. The adaptive value of this may be that the systems in the brain that implement the type of information processing involved in conscious thoughts are not interrupted by small signals that could be noise in sensory pathways. Then oscillations are argued to not be a necessary part of the implementation of consciousness in the brain.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 299.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 379.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 379.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Second order thoughts are thoughts about thoughts. Higher order thoughts refer to second order, third order etc. thoughts about thoughts… (A thought may be defined briefly as an intentional mental state, that is a mental state that is about something. Thoughts include beliefs, and are usually described as being propositional (Rosenthal DM (2005) Consciousness and Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press). An example of a thought is “It is raining”. A more detailed definition is as follows. A thought may be defined as an occurrent mental state (or event) that is intentional - that is a mental state that is about something - and also propositional, so that it is evaluable as true or false. Thoughts include occurrent beliefs or judgements. An example of a thought would be an occurrent belief that the earth moves around the sun/ that Maurice’s boat goes faster with two sails/ that it never rains in southern California.)

References

  • Aggelopoulos NC, Rolls ET (2005) Natural scene perception: inferior temporal cortex neurons encode the positions of different objects in the scene. European Journal of Neuroscience 22:2903–2916.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aggelopoulos NC, Franco L, Rolls ET (2005) Object perception in natural scenes: encoding by inferior temporal cortex simultaneously recorded neurons. Journal of neurophysiology 93:1342–1357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allport A (1988) What concept of consciousness? In: Consciousness in Contemporary Science (Marcel AJ, Bisiach E, eds), pp 159–182. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong DM, Malcolm N (1984) Consciousness and Causality. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Azzopardi P, Cowey A (1997) Is blindsight like normal, near-threshold vision? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 94:14190–14194.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baars BJ (1988) A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow HB (1997) Single neurons, communal goals, and consciousness. In: Cognition, Computation, and Consciousness (Ito M, Miyashita Y, Rolls ET, eds), pp 121–136. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Battaglia FP, Treves A (1998) Stable and rapid recurrent processing in realistic auto-associative memories. Neural Computation 10:431–450.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block N (1995) On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18:227–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block N (2005) Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9:46–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Booth DA (1985) Food-conditioned eating preferences and aversions with interoceptive elements: learned appetites and satieties. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 443:22–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers P (1996) Language, Thought and Consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers DJ (1996) The Conscious Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1990) How Monkeys See the World. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooney JW, Gazzaniga MS (2003) Neurological disorders and the structure of human consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:161–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick FHC, Koch C (1990) Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in the Neurosciences 2:263–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio AR (1994) Descartes’ Error. New York: Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies MK (2008) Consciousness and explanation. In: Frontiers of Consciousness (Weiskrantz L, Davies MK, eds), pp 1–54. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins R (1986) The Blind Watchmaker. Harlow: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins R (1989) The Selfish Gene, 2nd Edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Gelder B, Vroomen J, Pourtois G, Weiskrantz L (1999) Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex. Neuroreport 10:3759–3763.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deco G, Rolls ET (2005a) Neurodynamics of biased competition and co-operation for attention: a model with spiking neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 94:295–313.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deco G, Rolls ET (2005b) Attention, short-term memory, and action selection: a unifying theory. Progress in Neurobiology 76:236–256.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deco G, Rolls ET (2005c) Synaptic and spiking dynamics underlying reward reversal in orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 15:15–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deco G, Rolls ET (2006) Decision-making and Weber’s Law: a neurophysiological model. European Journal of Neuroscience 24:901–916.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deco G, Rolls ET, Romo R (2009) Stochastic dynamics as a principle of brain function. Progress in Neurobiology 88:1–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Naccache L (2001) Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79:1–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Changeux JP, Naccache L, Sackur J, Sergent C (2006) Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10:204–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett DC (1991) Consciousness Explained. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett DC (2005) Sweet Dreams: Philosophical Obstacles to a Science of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dinse HR, Kruger K (1994) The timing of processing along the visual pathway in the cat. Neuroreport 5:893–897.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliffe MCM, Rolls ET, Stringer SM (2002) Invariant recognition of feature combinations in the visual system. Biological Cybernetics 86:59–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor JA (1994) The Elm and the Expert: mentalese and its semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franco L, Rolls ET, Aggelopoulos NC, Treves A (2004) The use of decoding to analyze the contribution to the information of the correlations between the firing of simultaneously recorded neurons. Experimental Brain Research 155:370–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga MS (1988) Brain modularity: towards a philosophy of conscious experience. In: Consciousness in Contemporary Science (Marcel AJ, Bisiach E, eds), pp 218–238. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga MS (1995) Consciousness and the cerebral hemispheres. In: The Cognitive Neurosciences (Gazzaniga MS, ed), pp 1392–1400. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga MS, LeDoux J (1978) The Integrated Mind. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gennaro RJ, ed (2004) Higher Order Theories of Consciousness. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Rakic PS (1996) The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 351:1445–1453.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale MA (2004) Perceiving the world and grasping it: dissociations between conscious and unconscious visual processing. In: The Cognitive Neurosciences III (Gazzaniga MS, ed), pp 1159–1172. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton W (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Journal of Theortical Biology 7:1–52.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WD (1996) Narrow Roads of Gene Land. New York: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hampton RR (2001) Rhesus monkeys know when they remember. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98:5359–5362.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hampton RR, Zivin A, Murray EA (2004) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) discriminate between knowing and not knowing and collect information as needed before acting. Animal Cognition 7:239–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes CM (2008) Beast machines? Questions of animal consciousness. In: Frontiers of Consciousness (Weiskrantz L, Davies M, eds), pp 259–274. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hornak J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, Morris RG, O’Doherty J, Bullock PR, Polkey CE (2003) Changes in emotion after circumscribed surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Brain 126:1691–1712.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hornak J, O’Doherty J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, Morris RG, Bullock PR, Polkey CE (2004) Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16:463–478.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey NK (1980) Nature’s psychologists. In: Consciousness and the Physical World (Josephson BD, Ramachandran VS, eds), pp 57–80. Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey NK (1986) The Inner Eye. London: Faber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys GW, Bruce V (1991) Visual Cognition. Hove, East Sussex: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huxter J, Burgess N, O’Keefe J (2003) Independent rate and temporal coding in hippocampal pyramidal cells. Nature 425:828–832.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Insabato A, Pannunzi M, Rolls ET, Deco G (2010) Confidence-related decision-making. Journal of Neurophysiology 104:539–547.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff R (2002) Foundations of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird PN (1988) The Computer and the Mind: An Introduction to Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kadohisa M, Rolls ET, Verhagen JV (2005) Neuronal representations of stimuli in the mouth: the primate insular taste cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and amygdala. Chemical Senses 30:401–419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kepecs A, Uchida N, Zariwala HA, Mainen ZF (2008) Neural correlates, computation and behavioural impact of decision confidence. Nature 455:227–231.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koch C, Preuschoff K (2007) Betting the house on consciousness. Nature Neuroscience 10:140–141.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs JR, Kacelnik A (1991) Decision Making. In: Behavioural Ecology, 3rd Edition (Krebs JR, Davies NB, eds), pp 105–136. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamme VAF, Roelfsema PR (2000) The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neuroscience 23:571–579.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux J (2008) Emotional coloration of consciousness: how feelings come about. In: Frontiers of Consciousness (Weiskrantz L, Davies M, eds), pp 69–130. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux JE (1996) The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libet B (2002) The timing of mental events: Libet’s experimental findings and their implications. Consciousness and Cognition 11:291–299; discussion 304–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malsburg Cvd (1990) A neural architecture for the representation of scenes. In: Brain Organization and Memory: Cells, Systems and Circuits (McGaugh JL, Weinberger NM, Lynch G, eds), pp 356–372. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcel AJ (1983a) Conscious and unconscious perception: an approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology 15:238–300.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marcel AJ (1983b) Conscious and unconscious perception: experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology 15:197–237.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McLeod P, Plunkett K, Rolls ET (1998) Introduction to Connectionist Modelling of Cognitive Processes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller EK, Cohen JD (2001) An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual review of neuroscience 24:167–202.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milner AD (2008) Conscious and unconscious visual processing in the human brain. In: Frontiers of Consciousness (Weiskrantz L, Davies M, eds), pp 169–214. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner AD, Goodale MA (1995) The Visual Brain in Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowak LG, Bullier J (1997) The timing of information transfer in the visual system. In: Extrastriate visual cortex in primates (Rockland KS, Kaas JH, Peters A, eds), pp 205–241. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panzeri S, Schultz SR, Treves A, Rolls ET (1999) Correlations and the encoding of information in the nervous system. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 266:1001–1012.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Panzeri S, Rolls ET, Battaglia F, Lavis R (2001) Speed of feedforward and recurrent processing in multilayer networks of integrate-and-fire neurons. Network: Computation in Neural Systems 12:423–440.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Persaud N, McLeod P, Cowey A (2007) Post-decision wagering objectively measures awareness. Nature Neuroscience 10:257–261.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petrides M (1996) Specialized systems for the processing of mnemonic information within the primate frontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 351:1455–1462.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps EA, LeDoux JE (2005) Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron 48:175–187.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ridley M (1993) The Red Queen: Sex and the Evolution of Human Nature. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (1990) A theory of emotion, and its application to understanding the neural basis of emotion. Cognition and Emotion 4:161–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (1995) A theory of emotion and consciousness, and its application to understanding the neural basis of emotion. In: The Cognitive Neurosciences (Gazzaniga MS, ed), pp 1091–1106. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (1997a) Brain mechanisms of vision, memory, and consciousness. In: Cognition, Computation, and Consciousness (Ito M, Miyashita Y, Rolls ET, eds), pp 81–120. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (1997b) Consciousness in neural networks? Neural Networks 10:1227–1240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (1999a) The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Neurocase 5:301–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (1999b) The Brain and Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2000) Précis of The Brain and Emotion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23:177–233.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2003) Consciousness absent and present: a neurophysiological exploration. Progress in Brain Research 144:95–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2004a) The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain and cognition 55:11–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2004b) A higher order syntactic thought (HOST) theory of consciousness. In: Higher-Order Theories of Consciousness: An Anthology (Gennaro RJ, ed), pp 137–172. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2005a) Consciousness absent or present: a neurophysiological exploration of masking. In: The First Half Second: The Microgenesis and Temporal Dynamics of Unconscious and Conscious Visual Processes (Ogmen H, Breitmeyer BG, eds), pp 89–108, chapter 106. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2005b) Emotion Explained. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2006) Brain mechanisms underlying flavour and appetite. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B 361:1123–1136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2007a) A computational neuroscience approach to consciousness. Neural Networks 20:962–982.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2007b) The representation of information about faces in the temporal and frontal lobes. Neuropsychologia 45:125–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2007c) The affective neuroscience of consciousness: higher order linguistic thoughts, dual routes to emotion and action, and consciousness. In: Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness (Zelazo P, Moscovitch M, Thompson E, eds), pp 831–859. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2008a)Emotion, higher order syntactic thoughts, and consciousness. In: Frontiers of Consciousness (Weiskrantz L, Davies MK, eds), pp 131–167. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2008b) Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making: A Unifying Computational Neuroscience Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2009) The anterior and midcingulate cortices and reward. In: Cingulate Neurobiology and Disease (Vogt BA, ed), pp 191–206. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET (2011) Neuroculture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Tovee MJ (1994) Processing speed in the cerebral cortex and the neurophysiology of visual masking. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 257:9–15.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Treves A (1998) Neural Networks and Brain Function. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Deco G (2002) Computational Neuroscience of Vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Stringer SM (2006) Invariant visual object recognition: a model, with lighting invariance. Journal of Physiology Paris 100:43–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Kesner RP (2006) A computational theory of hippocampal function, and empirical tests of the theory. Progress in Neurobiology 79:1–48.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: from affect to decision-making. Progress in Neurobiology 86:216–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Deco G (2010) The Noisy Brain: Stochastic Dynamics as a Principle of Brain Function. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Tovee MJ, Panzeri S (1999) The neurophysiology of backward visual masking: information analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11:335–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F, Deco G (2010a) Choice, difficulty, and confidence in the brain. NeuroImage 53:694–706.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F, Deco G (2010) Decision-making, errors, and confidence in the brain. Journal of Neurophysiology 104:2359–2374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Hornak J, Wade D, McGrath J (1994a) Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 57:1518–1524.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Franco L, Aggelopoulos NC, Reece S (2003) An information theoretic approach to the contributions of the firing rates and correlations between the firing of neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 89:2810–2822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Aggelopoulos NC, Franco L, Treves A (2004) Information encoding in the inferior temporal cortex: contributions of the firing rates and correlations between the firing of neurons. Biological Cybernetics 90:19–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Franco L, Aggelopoulos NC, Perez JM (2006) Information in the first spike, the order of spikes, and the number of spikes provided by neurons in the inferior temporal visual cortex. Vision Research 46:4193–4205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls ET, Tovee MJ, Purcell DG, Stewart AL, Azzopardi P (1994b) The responses of neurons in the temporal cortex of primates, and face identification and detection. Experimental Brain Research 101:473–484.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal DM (1986) Two concepts of consciousness. Philosophical Studies 49:329–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal DM (1990) A Theory of Consciousness. Bielefeld, Germany: Zentrum für Interdisziplinaire Forschung.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal DM (1993) Thinking that one thinks. In: Consciousness (Davies M, Humphreys GW, eds), pp 197–223. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal DM (2004) Varieties of Higher-Order Theory. In: Higher Order Theories of Consciousness (Gennaro RJ, ed). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal DM (2005) Consciousness and Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart DE, Hinton GE, Williams RJ (1986) Learning internal representations by error propagation. In: Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition (Rumelhart DE, McClelland JL, Group TPR, eds). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice T, Burgess P (1996) The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 351:1405–1411.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simmen MW, Treves A, Rolls ET (1996) Pattern retrieval in threshold-linear associative nets. Network (Bristol, England) 7:109–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer W (1999) Neuronal synchrony: A versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron 24:49–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smerieri A, Rolls ET, Feng J (2010) Decision time, slow inhibition, and theta rhythm. Journal of Neuroscience 30:14173–14181.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith-Swintosky VL, Plata-Salaman CR, Scott TR (1991) Gustatory neural encoding in the monkey cortex: stimulus quality. Journal of Neurophysiology 66:1156–1165.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith PL, Ratcliff R (2004) Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions. Trends in Neurosciences 27:161–168.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Squire LR, Zola SM (1996) Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 93:13515–13522.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sugase Y, Yamane S, Ueno S, Kawano K (1999) Global and fine information coded by single neurons in the temporal visual cortex. Nature 400:869–873.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tovee MJ, Rolls ET (1992) The functional nature of neuronal oscillations. Trends in Neurosciences 15:387.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tovee MJ, Rolls ET (1995) Information encoding in short firing rate epochs by single neurons in the primate temporal visual cortex. Visual Cognition 2:35–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tovee MJ, Rolls ET, Treves A, Bellis RP (1993) Information encoding and the responses of single neurons in the primate temporal visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 70:640–654.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman A (1996) The binding problem. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 6:171–178.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treves A (1993) Mean-field analysis of neuronal spike dynamics. Network (Bristol, England) 4:259–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang XJ (2002) Probabilistic decision making by slow reverberation in cortical circuits. Neuron 36:955–968.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiskrantz L (1997) Consciousness Lost and Found. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiskrantz L (1998) Blindsight. A Case Study and Implications, 2nd Edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiskrantz L (2001) Blindsight – putting beta (β) on the back burner. In: Out of Mind: Varieties of Unconscious Processes (De Gelder B, De Haan E, Heywood C, eds), pp 20–31. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong KF, Wang XJ (2006) A recurrent network mechanism of time integration in perceptual decisions. Journal of Neuroscience 26:1314–1328.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yaxley S, Rolls ET, Sienkiewicz ZJ (1990) Gustatory responses of single neurons in the insula of the macaque monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology 63:689–700.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The author acknowledges helpful discussions with Martin Davies, Marian Dawkins and David Rosenthal.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Edmund T. Rolls .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Rolls, E.T. (2011). Consciousness, Decision-Making and Neural Computation. In: Cutsuridis, V., Hussain, A., Taylor, J. (eds) Perception-Action Cycle. Springer Series in Cognitive and Neural Systems. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1452-1_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics