Skip to main content

Mood as Information: The Regulatory Role of Personality

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: The Springer Series on Human Exceptionality ((SSHE))

Abstract

According to the mood-as-input model (Martin, 2001), moods operate much like any other piece of information. They serve as input in a configural processing system. Moods are processed in parallel with the target and contextual information in such a way that the meaning of the mood influences and is influenced by the meaning of other information. It is possible for either negative or positive feelings to convey either negative or positive evaluative and motivational implications, the nature of which depends on the context. Hence, the influence of mood on one’s evaluations, motivations, and behaviors depends on the interaction of mood and situational conditions (which is the so-called context-dependent effect of mood), suggesting, for example, that the relation between the mood and the way of processing information (systematic vs. heuristic) differs depending on the interaction of mood and the context of the task.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   69.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   89.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abele, A., & Petzold, P. (1994). How does mood operate in an impression formation task? An information integration approach. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 173–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allik, J., & Realo, A. (1997). Emotional experience and its relation to five-factor model in Estonian. Journal of Personality, 65, 625–647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allred, K. D., & Smith, T. W. (1991). Social cognition in cynical hostility. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15, 399–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., Clore, G. L., Golisano, V., Rabel, C., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Mood and the use of scripts: Do happy moods really make people mindless? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 665–678.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., Mackie, D., & Schwarz, N. (1992). Mood effects on encoding and judgmental processes in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 585–595.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., Schwarz, N., & Wieland, R. (1996). Mood and the impact of category membership and individuating information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 935–959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bodenhausen, G., Kramer, G., & Susser, K. (1994). Happiness and stereotypic thinking in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 621–632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H., Montiero, K. P., & Gilligan, S. G. (1978). Emotional mood as a context for learning and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 573–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B. P., & Mogg, K. (1994). Mood and personality in recall of positive and negative information. Behavior Research and Therapy, 31, 137–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, M., & Mathews, A. (1992). Autobiographical memory and clinical anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 23–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ciarrochi, J. V., & Forgas, J. P. (1999). On being tense yet tolerant: The paradoxical effects of trait anxiety and aversive mood on intergroup judgments. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research and Practice, 3, 227–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ciarrochi, J. V., & Forgas, J. P. (2000). The pleasure of possessions: Affective influences and personality in the evaluation of consumer items. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 631–649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L. (1992). Cognitive phenomenology: Feelings and the construction of judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 133–163). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., Wyer, R. S., Dienes, B., Gasper, K., Gohm, C., & Isbell, L. (2001). Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. In L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition (pp. 27–62). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eliasz, A. (1990). Broadening the concept of temperament: From disposition to hypothetical construct. European Journal of Personality, 4, 287–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eliasz, A., & Klonowicz, T. (2001). Top-down and bottom-up approaches to personality and their application to temperament. In A. Eliasz & A. Angleitner (Eds.), Advances in research on temperament (pp. 14–42). Lengerich: Pabst.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W. (1992). Anxiety: The cognitive perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1998). On being happy and mistaken: Mood effects on the fundamental attribution error. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 318–331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Behavioral activation and inhibition in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1135–1149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goryńska, E. (2005). Przymiotnikowa Skala Nastroju UMACL Geralda Matthewsa, A. Grahama Chamberlaina, Dylana M. Jonesa. Podręcznik [Gerald Matthews’, A. Graham Chamberlain’s, Dylan M. Jones’ mood adjective check list UMACL manual]. Warsaw: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych Polskiego Towarzystwa Psychologicznego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for personality (pp. 246–276). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Heubeck, B. G., Wilkinson, R. B., & Cologen, J. (1998). A second look at Carver and White’s (1994) BIS/BAS scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 785–800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., & Rholes, W. S. (1976). Impression formation and role fulfillment: A “holistic reference” approach. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 422–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E., Bernet, C. Z., & McLaughlin, S. C. (1994). Attentional allocation processes in individuals at risk for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 317–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory and behavior: A cognitive loop? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Henderson, A. S., Jacomb, P. A., Korten, A. E., & Rodgers, B. (1999). Using the BIS/BAS scales to measure behavioral inhibition and behavioral activation: Factor structure, validity and norms in a large community sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 49–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Josephson, B. R., Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Mood regulation and memory: Repairing sad moods with happy memories. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 437–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kammer, D. (1994). On depression and state orientation: A few empirical and theoretical remarks. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 351–362). Göttingen/Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanfer, R., Dugdale, B., & McDonald, B. (1994). Empirical findings on the Action Control Scale in the context of complex skill acquisition. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 61–77). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufmann, G., & Vosburg, S. K. (1997). Paradoxial mood effects on creative problem solving. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 151–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufmann, G., & Vosburg, S. K. (2002). Effects of mood on early and late idea production. Creativity Research Journal, 14, 317–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kolańczyk, A. (2004). Stany uwagi sprzyjające wpływom afektu na ocenianie [States of attention fostering influences of affect on judgments]. Studia Psychologiczne, 42(1), 93–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional mediators of cognitive-behavior consistency: Self-regulatory process and action versus state orientation. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 101–128). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1986). Motivation and information processing: A new look at decision making, dynamic change, and action control. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation metacognition, foundations of social behavior (pp. 404–434). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1994). A theory of action and state orientations. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 9–46). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem-solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marszał-Wiśniewska, M. (1999). Siła Woli a Temperament [Will power and temperament]. Warsaw: Wydawnictwo Instytutu Psychologii PAN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marszał-Wiśniewska, M. (2001). Self-regulatory abilities, temperament, and volition in everyday life situations. In H. Brandstätter & A. Eliasz (Eds.), Persons, situations, and emotions: An ecological approach (pp. 74–94). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marszał-Wiśniewska, M. (2002). Adaptacja Skali Kontroli Działania J. Kuhla (ACS-90) [Adaptation of action control scale by J. Kuhl]. Studia Psychologiczne, 40(2), 77–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marszał-Wiśniewska, M., & Zajusz, D. (2005). The informative function of mood: The modifying role of depressive tendencies and temperament. Warsaw School of Social Psychology: unpublished data.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L. (2001). Mood as input: A configural view of mood effect. In J. P. Forgast (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 135–157). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., Abend, T., Sedikides, C., & Green, J. D. (1997). How would I feel if...? Mood as input to a role fulfillment evaluation process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 242–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., & Clore, G. L. (2001). Theories of mood and cognition. A user’s guidebook. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., & Davies, B. (1998). Beyond hedonism and associationism: A configural view of the role of affect in self-regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 33–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., & Stoner, P. (1996). Mood as input: What we think about how we feel determines how we think. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect and self-regulation (pp. 279–301). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., Ward, D. W., Achee, J. W., & Wyer, R. S. (1993). Mood as input: People have to interpret the motivational implications of their moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 317–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Davies, D. R., & Holley, P. J. (1990). Extraversion, arousal, and visual sustained attention: The role of resource availability. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 1159–1173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Deary, I. J., & Whiteman, M. C. (2003). Personality traits. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Jones, D. M., & Chamberlain, A. G. (1990). Refining the measurement of mood: The UWIST mood adjective checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braverman, D. L., & Evans, T. W. (1992). Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 119–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., McCormick, L. J., & Strong, S. E. (1995). Mood-congruent memory and natural mood: New evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 736–746.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. A., & Setterlund, M. B. (1994). Emotion congruence in perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 401–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pervin, L. A. (1976). A free-response description approach to the analysis of person-situation interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 465–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., Schumann, D. W., Richman, S. A., & Strathman, A. (1993). Positive mood and persuasion: Different roles for affect under high and low elaboration conditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 5–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pusateri, T. P., & Latane, B. (1982). Respect and admiration: Evidence for configural information integration of achieved and ascribed characteristics. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 87–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, M. A., & Derryberry, D. (1995). Temperament and attention to positive and negative trait information. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 135–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, A., French, C. C., Johnson, W., Naparstek, J., & Williams, J. (1992). Effects of mood manipulation and anxiety on performance of an emotional Stroop task. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 479–491.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rusting, C. L. (1998). Personality, mood, and cognitive processing of emotional information: Three conceptual frameworks. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 165–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rusting, C. L., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Regulating responses to anger: Effects of rumination and distraction on angry mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 790–803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527–561). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N. (2001). Feelings as information: Implications for affective influences on information processing. In L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s handbook (pp. 159–176). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Bless, B. (1991). Happy and mindless, but sad and smart? The impact of afective states on analytic reasoning. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgments (pp. 55–71). London: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., Bless, H., & Bohner, G. (1991). Mood and persuasion: Affective states influence the processing of persuasive communications. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 161–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Bohner, G. (1996). Feelings and their motivational implications: Moods and the action sequence. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 119–145). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1988). How do I feel about it? The informative function of mood. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and social behavior (pp. 44–62). Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1996). Feelings and phenomental experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: A handbook of basic principles (pp. 443–465). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Personality moderators of mood congruency effects on cognition: The role of self-esteem and negative mood regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1092–1107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strelau, J. (1993). The location of the regulative theory of temperament (RTT) among other temperament theories. In J. Hettema & I. J. Deary (Eds.), Foundations of personality (pp. 113–132). Dordrecht: Kluver.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Strelau, J. (1996). The regulative theory of temperament: Current status. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 131–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strelau, J. (1998). Temperament: A psychological perspective. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strelau, J., & Zawadzki, B. (1995). The formal characteristics of behaviour – Temperament inventory (FCB – TI): validity studies. European Journal of Personality, 9, 207–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szymura, B., & Nęcka, E. (2005). Three superfactors of personality and three aspects of attention. In A. Eliasz, S. E. Hampson, & B. de Raad (Eds.), Advances in personality psychology (pp. 75–90). Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1993). Controlling anger: Self-induced emotion change. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control (pp. 393–409). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect. The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Worth, L. T., & Mackie, D. M. (1987). Cognitive mediation of positive affect in persuasion. Social Cognition, 5, 76–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, W. F., & Bower, G. H. (1992). Mood effects on subjective probability assessment. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 52, 276–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Magdalena Marszał-Wiśniewska .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Marszał-Wiśniewska, M., Zajusz, D. (2010). Mood as Information: The Regulatory Role of Personality. In: Gruszka, A., Matthews, G., Szymura, B. (eds) Handbook of Individual Differences in Cognition. The Springer Series on Human Exceptionality. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1210-7_23

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics