Skip to main content

The Unique Cognitive Limitation in Subclinical Depression: The Impairment of Mental Model Construction

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Handbook of Individual Differences in Cognition

Abstract

The notion of “depression” is frequently employed to describe a broader category of depressive symptoms, dysphoria, and the depression syndrome as such (Joormann, 2005). Numerous debates in the literature have addressed the issue of continuity, the question of whether moderate depression symptoms (called subclinical depression) differ quantitatively or qualitatively from severe clinical depression. Flett, Vredenburg, and Kramses (1997) drew up a summary indicating that the available data is generally consistent with the hypothesis of continuity. In this chapter, we review data from studies in which subclinical forms of depressive disorders were taken into account: those that are mild in terms of severity. Depression is then seen as an affective disorder characterized by persistent negative mood (without an elevated level of arousal) and specific deficits in cognitive functioning. These deficits include “ruminative” thinking, recurring ideas and thoughts with negative or self-devaluing content. Such deficits also involve individuals with depression experiencing difficulty in solving complex cognitive problems, and solving problems that require reasoning about deeper social relations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 69.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 89.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Acuna, B. D., Eliassen, J. C., Donoghue, J. P., & Sanes, J. N. (2002). Frontal and parietal lobe activation during transitive inference in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 1312–1321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, W. F. (1987). Schemas versus mental models in human memory. In P. Morris (Ed.), Modelling cognition (pp. 187–197). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bucciarelli, M., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1999). Strategies in syllogistic reasoning. Cognitive Science, 23, 247–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, D. B., Zembar, M. J., & Niederehe, G. (1995). Depression and memory impairment: A meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 285–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Channon, S., Baker, J. E., & Robertson, M. M. (1993). Effects of structure and clustering on recall and recognition memory in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 323–326.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (1996). Individual differences in working memory capacity: More evidence for a general capacity theory. Memory, 4, 577–590.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M., & Giannopoulos, C. (1993). Dysphoria and decision making: Limited information use for evaluation of multiattribute targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 613–623.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Copeland, D. E., & Radvansky, G. A. (2004). Working memory and syllogistic reasoning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1437–1457.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, C. G., Wortman, C. B., Silver, R. C., & Thompson, S. C. (1995). The undoing of traumatic life events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 109–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H. C., & Ashbrook, P. W. (1988). Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states on memory. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition and social behavior (pp. 25–43). Toronto: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H. C., Ottaway, S. A., Varner, L. J., Becker, A. S., & Moore, B. A. (1997). Emotion, motivation, and text comprehension: The detection of contradictions in passages. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 131–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., Sedek, G., von Hecker, U., & McIntosh, D. N. (Eds.). (2005). Cognitive limitations in aging and psychopathology. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fangmeier, T., Knauff, M., Ruff, C. C., & Sloutsky, V. (2006). fMRI evidence for a three-stage model of deductive reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 320–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flett, G. L., Vredenburg, K., & Kramses, L. (1997). The continuity of depression in clinical and nonclinical samples. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 395–416.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gannon, K. M., Skowronski, J. J., & Betz, A. L. (1994). Depressive diligence in social information processing: Implications for order effects in impressions and for social memory. Social Cognition, 12, 263–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garnham, A. (1997). Representing information in mental models. In M. A. Conway (Ed.), Cognitive models of memory (pp. 149–172). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilinski, A., & Judd, B. B. (1994). Working memory bias in reasoning across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 3, 356–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & Hammen, C. (1992). Psychological aspect of depression: Toward an interpersonal integration. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., Roberts, J. E., & Gilboa, E. (1996). Cognitive interference in depression. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference: Theories, methods and findings (pp. 347–377). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. (1989). Situations, mental models, and generative knowledge. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing (pp. 285–318). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Vazquez, C., & Dykman, D. (1993). Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 247–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958). The Psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T. (1997). On the contributions of deficient cognitive control to memory impairments in depression. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 569–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T. (2004). Memory for emotional and nonemotional events in depression: A question of habit? In D. Reisberg & P. Hertel (Eds.), Memory and emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T., & Hardin, T. S. (1990). Remembering with and without awareness in depressed mood: Evidence of deficits in initiative. Journal Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 45–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T., & Rude, S. S. (1991). Depressive deficits in memory: Focusing attention improves subsequent recall. Journal Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 301–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, E., von Hecker, U., Singh, K., & Wise, R. (2008). Generative reasoning in subclinical depression: Exploring the neural basis. Unpublished manuscript, Cardiff University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. H., Holyoak, K. J., Nisbett, R. E., & Thagard, P. R. (1986). Induction: Processes of inference, learning and discovery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hummert, M. L., Crockett, W. H., & Kemper, S. (1990). Processing mechanisms underlying use of the balance schema. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 5–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J. (1968). Constructing spatial images: A strategy in reasoning. Psychological Review, 75, 550–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference and consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1996). Images, models, and propositional representations. In M. de Vega, M. J. Intons-Peterson, P. N. Johnson-Laird, M. Denis, & M. Marschark (Eds.), Models of visuospatial cognition (pp. 90–127). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J. (2005). Inhibition, rumination, and mood regulation in depression. In R. W. Engle, G. Sedek, U. von Hecker, & D. N. McIntosh (Eds.), Cognitive limitations in aging and psychopathology (pp. 275–312). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knauff, M., Mulack, T., Kassubek, J., Salih, H. R., & Greenlee, M. W. (2002). Spatial imagery in deductive reasoning: A functional MRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 203–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kofta, M., & Sedek, G. (1989). Repeated failure: A source of helplessness, or a factor irrelevant to its emergence? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 3–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kofta, M., & Sedek, G. (1998). Uncontrollability as a source of cognitive exhaustion: Implications for helplessness and depression. In M. Kofta, G. Weary, & G. Sedek (Eds.), Personal control in action: Cognitive and motivational mechanisms (pp. 391–418). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kofta, M., & Sedek, G. (1999). Uncontrollability as irreducible uncertainty. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 577–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroger, J. K., Sabb, F. W., Fales, C. L., Bookheimer, S. Y., Cohen, M. S., & Holyoak, K. J. (2002). Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: A parametric study of relational complexity. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 477–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1984). Volitional aspects of achievement motivation and learned helplessness: Toward a comprehensive theory of action control. In B. Maher (Ed.), Progress of experimental personality research (Vol. 13, pp. 99–171). San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linville, P. (1996). Attention inhibition: Does it underlie ruminative thought? In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 9, pp. 121–133). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martindale, C. (1977). Creativity, consciousness, and cortical arousal. Journal of Altered States of Consciousness, 3, 69–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marx, E. M., Williams, J. M. G., & Claridge, G. (1992). Depression and social problem solving. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 78–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIntosh, D. N., Sedek, G., Fojas, S., Brzezicka-Rotkiewicz, A., & Kofta, M. (2005). Cognitive performance after preexposure to uncontrollability and in depressive state: Going with a simpler “Plan B”. In R. W. Engle, G. Sedek, U. von Hecker, & D. N. McIntosh (Eds.), Cognitive limitations in aging and psychopathology (pp. 219–246). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Tangney, J. P., & Gavanski, I. (1994). “If I only weren’t” versus “if only I hadn’t”: Distinguishing shame and guilt in counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 585–595.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 29–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1974). The child’s construction of quantities: Conservation and atomism. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pittman, T. S., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1989). Motivation and cognition: Control deprivation and the nature of subsequent information processing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 465–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potts, G. R. (1972). Information processing strategies used in the encoding of linear orderings. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 727–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radvansky, G. A., & Copeland, D. E. (2004). Reasoning, integration, inference alteration, and text comprehension. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 133–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radvansky, G. A., Zwaan, R. A., Curiel, J. M., & Copeland, D. E. (2001). Situation models and aging. Psychology and Aging, 16, 145–160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T. A. (2001). Attempted decomposition of age-related influences on two tests of reasoning. Psychology and Aging, 16, 251–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmalhofer, F., & Glavanow, D. (1986). Three components of understanding a programmer’s manual: Verbatim, propositional, and situational representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 279–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sedek, G., & Kofta, M. (1990). When cognitive exertion does not yield cognitive gain: Toward an informational explanation of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 729–743.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sedek, G., Kofta, M., & Tyszka, T. (1993). Effects of uncontrollability on subsequent decision making: Testing the cognitive exhaustion hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1270–1281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sedek, G., & McIntosh, D. N. (1998). Intellectual helplessness: Domain specificity, teaching styles, and school achievement. In M. Kofta, G. Weary, & G. Sedek (Eds.), Personal control in action: Cognitive and motivational mechanisms (pp. 391–418). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedek, G., Oberauer, K., & von Hecker, U. (2008). Influence of depression and working memory on belief bias and categorical reasoning. Unpublished manuscript, Warsaw School of Social Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedek, G., & von Hecker, U. (2004). Effects of subclinical depression and aging on generative reasoning about linear orders: Same or different processing limitations? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 237–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sedek, G., & Zientecka (2008). Situation models and depression. Unpublished manuscript, Warsaw School of Social Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development and death. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. D., Tracy, J. I., & Murray, M. J. (1993). Depression and category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 331–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 34–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1980). Representation and process in linear syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 119–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Hecker, U. (1997). How do logical inference rules help construct social mental models? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 367–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Hecker, U., & Meiser, T. (2005). Defocused attention in depressed mood: Evidence from source monitoring. Emotion, 5, 456–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Hecker, U., & Sedek, G. (1999). Uncontrollability, depression, and the construction of mental models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 833–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Hecker, U., Sedek, G., & McIntosh, D. N. (2000). Impaired systematic, higher order strategies in depression and helplessness: Testing implications of the cognitive exhaustion model. In U. von Hecker, S. Dutke, & G. Sedek (Eds.), Generative mental processes and cognitive resources: Integrative research on adaptation and control (pp. 245–276). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Watts, F. N., & Cooper, Z. (1989). The effects of structural aspects of the recall of prose. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 150–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., Marsh, K. L., Gleicher, F., & Edwards, J. A. (1993). Depression, control motivation, and the processing of information about others. In G. Weary, F. Gleicher, & K. L. Marsh (Eds.), Control motivation and social cognition (pp. 255–287). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Weingartner, H. (1986). Automatic and effort demanding cognitive processes in depression. In L. W. Poon et al. (Eds.), Handbook for clinical memory assessment of older adults (pp. 218–225). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this article was supported by MNiSW grants N N106040534 and N106 017 31/1344.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Grzegorz Sedek .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sedek, G., Brzezicka, A., von Hecker, U. (2010). The Unique Cognitive Limitation in Subclinical Depression: The Impairment of Mental Model Construction. In: Gruszka, A., Matthews, G., Szymura, B. (eds) Handbook of Individual Differences in Cognition. The Springer Series on Human Exceptionality. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1210-7_20

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics