Skip to main content

The Role of Hebrew Letter Names in Early Literacy: The Case of Multi-phonemic Acrophonic Names

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Literacy Development and Enhancement Across Orthographies and Cultures

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 2))

Abstract

English-speaking children more often spell letters correctly when the letters’ names are heard in the word (e.g., B in beach vs. bone). Hebrew letter names have been claimed to be less useful in this regard. In Study 1, kindergartners were asked to report and spell the initial and final letters in Hebrew words, including full (CVC), partial (CV), and phonemic (C) cues derived from these letter names (e.g., kaftor, kartis, kibεl, spelled with /kaf/). Correct and biased responses increased with the length of congruent and incongruent cues, respectively. In Study 2, preschoolers and kindergartners were asked to report the initial letters of monosyllabic or disyllabic names (e.g., /kaf/ and /samεx/, respectively) that included the cues described above. Correct responses increased with cue length; the effect was stronger with monosyllabic over disyllabic letter names, probably because the cue covered a larger ratio of the letter name. Phonological awareness was linked to the use of letter names.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bowman, M., & Treiman, R. (2002). Relating print and speech: the effects of letter naming and word position on reading and spelling performance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 305–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, S. R., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Bidirectional relations of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: evidence from preschool sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 117–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., Genesee, F., & Caravolas, M. (1997). A cross-linguistic study of early literacy acquisition. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: implications for early interventions (pp. 145–162). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. (1998). The foundations of literacy: the child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Hove, United Kingdom: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The foundations of spelling ability: evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 751–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardoso-Martins, C. (1995). Sensitivity to rhymes, syllables, and phonemes in literacy acquisition in Portuguese. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 808–828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardoso-Martins, C., Lemos, L. S., Corrêa, M. F., & Napoleão, R. F. (2006). Is there a syllabic stage in the development of spelling? Evidence from Brazilian Portuguese-speaking children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 628–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardoso-Martins, C., Resende, S. M., & Rodrigues, L. A. (2002). Letter name knowledge and the ability to learn to read by processing letter-phoneme relations in words: evidence from Brazilian-Portuguese-speaking children. Reading and Writing, 15, 409–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Abreu, M. D., & Cardoso-Martins, C. (1998). Alphabetic access route in beginning reading acquisition in Portuguese: the role of letter-name knowledge. Reading and Writing, 10, 85–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In P. Gough, L. Ehri & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107–143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (1998). Learning to read and learning to spell are one and the same, almost. In C. Perfetti, L. Rieben & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell (pp. 237–269). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L., & Wilce, L. (1985). Movement into reading: is the first stage of printed word learning visual or phonetic? Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 163–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., Borstrom, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: the importance of distinctness of phonological representation of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 36–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, M. A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 65–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foulin, J. A. (2005). Why is letter-name knowledge such a good predictor of learning to read? Reading and Writing, 18, 129–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Patel, S., Margalit, T., & Barad, N. (2002). Letter-names: effect on letter saying, spelling, and word recognition in Hebrew. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 269–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Shatil-Carmon, S., & Asif-Rave, O. (2005). Learning of letter names and sounds and contribution to word reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 139–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of early literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1999). The ABC’s of the ABC’s: the development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 285–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Treiman, R. (2003). Hong Kong Chinese kindergartners learn to read English analytically. Psychological Science, 14, 138–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Taylor, S. (1997). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 370–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Lefty, D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development, 72, 816–833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (2001). Learning to spell in Hebrew: phonological and morphological factors. Reading and Writing, 14, 459–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (2004). Hebrew orthography and literacy. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 339–363). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Accardo & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability: a view of the spectrum (pp. 75–119). Timonium, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: a longitudinal comparative study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, J., & Ehri, L. (1990). Sight word reading in prereaders: use of logographic vs. alphabetic access routes. Journal of Reading Behavior, 22, 149–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Senechal, M., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: a five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 445–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Blum, P. (2005). Syllable splitting in literate and pre-literate Hebrew speakers: onsets and rimes or bodies and codas. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 182–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., MacLean, R., & Matthews, R. (1984). Sources of individual differences in reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1309–1324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shimron, J. (2007). Tnu lahem likro. Hed Hachinuch, 81, 58–63. Let them read, The Echo of Education, 81, 58–63. (Hebrew)

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: individual differences in precursors of reading skills. Child Development, 74, 358–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stage, S. A., & Wagner, R. K. (1992). The development of young children’s phonological and orthographic knowledge as revealed by their spellings. Developmental Psychology, 28, 287–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: evidence from a longitudinal model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934–947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L. (2003). The cradle of culture and what children know about writing and numbers before being taught. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Berch, D., & Weatherston, S. (1993). Children’s use of grapheme-phoneme correspondences in spelling: roles of position and stress. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Levin, I., & Kessler, B. (2007). Learning of letter names follows similar principles across languages: evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 87–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Rodriguez, K. (1999). Young children use letter names in learning to read words. Psychological Science, 10, 334–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Sotak, L., & Bowman, M. (2001). The roles of letter names and letter sounds in connecting print and speech. Memory and Cognition, 29, 860–873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Tincoff, R., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1996). Letter names help children connect print and speech. Developmental Psychology, 32, 505–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: a 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am very grateful to Bracha Nir-Sagiv for her contribution to the analysis of a corpus of words in children’s books, for counting letter name cues. Thanks are also extended to Yafit Melamed and Danit Ben-Amar for their involvement in the empirical study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Iris Levin .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendices

Appendix 5.A. Words Used for Tests of Letter Reporting, Letter Spelling, and Phoneme Isolation, in Study 1

Cue function

Cue length – initial position

Cue length – final position

Congruent

Letter name cue

Full letter name

Partial letter name

Phonemic

Letter name cue

Full letter name

Partial letter name

Phonemic

(CVC)

(CV)

(C)

(CVC)

(VC)

(C)

/Sin/

Sinuj

Siul

Saxor

/mεm/

ximεm

Silεm

olam

(ןיש)

(Change)

(Cough)

(Black)

 

(Warmed)

(Paid)

(World)

ש

יוניש

לועיש

רוחש

ם

םמחמ

םליש

םלוע

/kaf/

kaftor

kartis

kivsa

/nun/

tsinun

tsijun

aSan

(ףכ)

(Button)

(Ticket)

(Ewe)

 

(Cold)

(Score)

(Smoke)

כ

רותפכ

סיטרכ

השבכ

ן

ןוניצ

ןויצ

ןשע

/taf/

tafkid

talmid

tikra

/tεt/

xitεt

Sakεt

paSut

(ףת)

(Role)

(Pupil)

(Ceiling)

 

(Dug)

(Quiet)

(Simple)

ת

דיקפת

דימלת

הרקת

ט

טטיח

טקש

טושפ

Incongruent

/kaf/

kafats

kaSar

kibεl

/kuf/

Sakuf

saruf

magaf

Spelled /kuf/

(Jumped)

(Tied)

(Received)

Spelled /fεi/

(Trans-parent)

(Burnt)

(Boot)

(ףוק)

   

(יפ)

   

ק

ץפק

רשק

לביק

ף

ףוקש

ףורש

ףגמ

/tεt/

tεtakεn

tεvakεS

torid

/xεt/

Soxεt

boεt

tsavat

Spelled /taf/

(Will repair)

(Will ask)

(Will bring down)

Spelled /tεt/

(Butcher)

(Kicks)

(Pinched)

(ףת)

   

(תט)

   

ת

ןקתת

שקבת

דירות

ט

טחוס

טעוב

טבצ

/taf/

tafsan

tardan

tilon

/taf/

kataf

anaf

Sotεf

Spelled /tet/

(Scaffolder)

(Trouble-maker)

(Popsicle)

Spelled /fεi/

(Picked)

(Branch)

(Fluent)

(תט)

   

(יפ)

   

ט

ןספט

ןדרט

ןוליט

ף

ףטק

ףנע

ףטוש

  1. Note: All these Hebrew words are stressed on the last syllable; Hebrew words are spelled from right to left

Appendix 5.B. Words Used for Test of Letter Reporting, in Study 2 (n = 34)

Letter name length

Cue length – initial position

Percentage of correct responses

Mono-syllabic

Letter name cue

CVC

CV

C

CVC

CV

C

/bet/

‘bεtεn

‘bεrεz

‘bokεr

73.5

64.7

35.3

(תיב)

(Belly)

(Faucet)

(Morning)

   

ב

ןטב

זרב

רקוב

   

/vav/

vavim

varod

vilon

91.2

79.4

61.8

(וו)

(Hooks)

(Pink)

(Curtain)

   

ו

םיוו

דורו

ןוליו

   

/xεt/

xεtεf’

xεvεl’

xoSεx’

73.5

58.8

29.4

(תיח)

(Snatch)

(Rope)

(Darkness)

   

ח

ףטח

לבח

ךשוח

   

/jud/

judrax

jugaS

jiStε

82.4

76.5

5.9

(דוי)

(Will-be-guided)

(Will-be-served)

(Will drink)

   

דוי

ךרדוי

שגוי

התשי

   

/kaf/

kaftor

kartis

koxav

67.7

38.2

17.7

(ףכ)

(Button)

(Ticket)

(Star)

   

כ

רותפכ

סיטרכ

בכוכ

   

/mem/

mεmSala

mεdina

miSpaxa

91.2

70.6

44.1

(םמ)

(Government)

(State)

(Family)

   

מ

הלשממ

הנידמ

החפשמ

   

/kuf/

kufsa

kulmus

kotzim

73.5

26.5

20.6

(ףוק)

(Box)

(Pen)

(Thorns)

   

ק

הספוק

סומלוק

םיצוק

   

/rεS/

‘rεSεt

‘rεxεv

‘ra’al

79.4

61.8

47.1

(שיר)

(Net)

(Car)

(Poison)

   

ר

תשר

בכר

לער

   

/Sin/

Sinuj

Sikun

Sa’on

85.3

73.5

55.9

ןיש

(Change)

(Neighborhood)

(Watch)

   

ש

יוניש

ןוכיש

ןועש

   

/taf/

tafkid

targil

tinok

73.5

32.4

11.8

(ףת)

(Role)

(Exercise)

(Baby)

   

ת

דיקפת

ליגרת

קונית

   

Di-syllabic

/gimεl/

gimur

giSur

gozal

58.8

50.0

23.5

(למיג)

(Finish)

(Bridging)

(Chick)

   

ג

רומיג

רושיג

לזוג

   

/dalεt/

dalik

dagim

doker

70.6

38.2

26.5

(תלד)

(Flammable)

(Fish)

(Pricking)

   

ד

קילד

םיגד

רקוד

   

/zajn/

‘zajit

‘za’am

‘zεvεl

70.6

64.7

47.1

(ןיז)

(Olive)

(Rage)

(Trash)

   

ז

תיז

םעז

לבז

   

/lamεd/

‘lama

‘laka

‘lεxεm

76.5

61.8

41.2

(דמל)

(Why)

(Nail polish)

(Bread)

   

ל

המל

הכל

םחל

   

/samεx/

samal

salat

sipur

47.1

50.0

38.2

(ךמס)

(Sergeant)

(Salad)

(Story)

   

ס

למס

טלס

רופיס

   

/ajin/

ajir’

Ako’

ovεsh’

64.7

20.6

8.8

(ןיע)

(Donkey)

(Acre)

(Mold)

   

ע

ריע

וכע

שבוע

   

/tsazik/

tsadu

tsamu

tzemer

76.5

35.3

52.9

(קידצ)

(Hunted)

(Fasted)

(Wool)

   

צ

ודצ

ומצ

רמצ

   
  1. Note: All Hebrew words marked by ‘ at the beginning are stressed on the first syllable. Other words are stressed on the last syllable; stress had no significant effect on frequency of correct responses either as a main effect or in interaction with cue length. Hebrew words are spelled from right to left.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Levin, I. (2010). The Role of Hebrew Letter Names in Early Literacy: The Case of Multi-phonemic Acrophonic Names. In: Aram, D., Korat, O. (eds) Literacy Development and Enhancement Across Orthographies and Cultures. Literacy Studies, vol 2. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0834-6_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics