Skip to main content

Domain-Generality Versus Domain-Specificity of Creativity

  • Chapter
Milieus of Creativity

Part of the book series: Knowledge and Space ((KNAS,volume 2))

If Einstein had trained as an artist, would he have been as creative a painter as Van Gogh? How about if Van Gogh had trained as a physicist—would he have been as creative as Einstein? One feels, in answering questions such as these, that the answer is probably “no.” But why? Certainly there are people who are multitalented. William Blake achieved great renown as both a writer and a painter. Leonardo da Vinci showed great talent in painting and invention. Richard Feynman is known primarily as a physicist, but his popular books achieved great renown. Is creativity the same or different across different domains, and why? This essay addresses the question of the domain-generality versus domain-specificity of creativity through a theoretical analysis of the construct. As argued in the following pages, there is no general answer to the question of whether creativity is domain-general or domain-specific; creativity varies across individuals as a function of three variables.

The greatest challenge in understanding the domain-generality versus specificity of creativity is in understanding the concept of a domain itself. Is literature a domain, or German literature, or modern German literature, or modern German literature in its original language, or what? Is cognitive psychology a domain, or psychology, or behavioral science, or social science? Because no consensual definition of a domain currently exists, it is impossible at this time to have a clear sense of exactly what domain-specificity means. Domains may themselves be defined at varying levels of generality or specificity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Amabile, T. M. (1983).The social psychology of creativity. New rk: ringer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. & Cattell, A. K. S. (1973).Measuring intelligence with the Culture Fair Tests. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996).Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. & Ryan, R. M. (1985).Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1999).Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development.Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frensch, P. A. & Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Expertise and intelligent thinking: When is it worse to know better? In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Advances in the psychology of human intelligence(Vol. 5, pp. 157–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, J. & Koelling, R. A. (1966). The relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning.Psychonomic Science,4, 123–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1993).Creating minds: An anatomy of creativity seen through the lives of Freud, Einstein, Picasso, Stravinsky, Eliot, Graham, and Gandhi. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity.American Psychologist,5, 444–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubart, T. I. (1994). Creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Thinking and problem solving(pp. 290–323). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lubart, T. I. & Sternberg, R. J. (1995). An investment approach to creativity: Theory and data. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.),The creative cognition approach(pp. 271–302). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, T. (1981).Tar baby. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niu, W.-H. & Sternberg, R. J. (2001). Cultural influences on artistic creativity and its evaluation.International Journal of Psychology, 36, 225–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O'Hara, L. A. & Sternberg, R. J. (2000–2001). It doesn't hurt to ask: Effects of instructions to be creative, practical, or analytical on essay-writing performance and their interaction with students' thinking styles.Creativity Research Journal,13, 197–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plath, S. (1963/1971).The bell jar. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubenson, D. L. & Runco, M. A. (1992). The psychoeconomic approach to creativity.New Ideas in Psychology,10, 131–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1988a). Creativity, leadership, and chance. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),The nature of creativity(pp. 386–426). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1988b).Scientific genius: A psychology of science. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1994).Greatness: Who makes history and why. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stemler, S. E., Grigorenko, E. L., Jarvin, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (2006). Using the theory of successful intelligence as a basis for augmenting AP exams in psychology and statistics.Contemporary Educational Psychology,31, 344–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1981). Intelligence and nonentrenchment.Journal of Educational Psychology,73, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1982). Natural, unnatural, and supernatural concepts.Cognitive Psychology,14, 451–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1985).Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1988). Mental self-government: A theory of intellectual styles and their development.Human Development,31, 197–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1993).Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test. Unpublished test.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1997a).Successful intelligence. New York: Plume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1997b).Thinking styles. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1999). A propulsion model of creative contributions.Review of General Psychology,3, 83–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (2001). Teaching psychology students that creativity is a decision.The General Psychologist,36, 8–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.) (2003a).Psychologists defying the crowd: Stories of those who battled the establishment and won. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (2003b).Wisdom, intelligence, and creativity synthesized. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (2005). WICS: A model of positive educational leadership comprising wisdom, intelligence, and creativity synthesized.Educational Psychology Review,17(3), 191–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. & Gastel, J. (1989a). Coping with novelty in human intelligence: An empirical investigation.Intelligence,17, 187–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. & Gastel, J. (1989b). If dancers ate their shoes: Inductive reasoning with factual and counterfactual premises.Memory and Cognition,17, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. & Grigorenko, E. L. (1995). Styles of thinking in school.European Journal for High Ability,6, 201–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. & Kalmar D. A. (1997). When will the milk spoil? Everyday induction in human intelligence.Intelligence,25, 185–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. & Lubart, T. I. (1991). An investment theory of creativity and its development.Human Development,34, 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. & Lubart, T. I. (1995).Defying the crowd: Cultivating creativity in a culture of conformity. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. & Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity.American Psychologist,51, 677–688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., Torff, B., & Grigorenko, E. L. (1998a). Teaching for successful intelligence raises school achievement.Phi Delta Kappan,79, 667–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., Torff, B., & Grigorenko, E. L. (1998b). Teaching triarchically improves school achievement.Journal of Educational Psychology,90, 374–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., Ferrari, M., & Clinkenbeard, P. (1999). A triarchic analysis of an aptitude-treatment interaction.European Journal of Psychological Assessment,15(1), 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2001). The propulsion model of creative contributions applied to the arts and letters.Journal of Creative Behavior,35, 75–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2002).The creativity conundrum. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. & The Rainbow Project Collaborators (2006). The Rainbow Project: Enhancing the SAT through assessments of analytical, practical, and creative skills.Intelligence,34, 321–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tetewsky, S. J. & Sternberg, R. J. (1986). Conceptual and lexical determinants of nonentrenched thinking.Journal of Memory and Language,5, 202–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, E. P. (1962).Guiding creative talent. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, E. P. (1974).Torrance tests of creative thinking: Norms-technical manual. Lexington, MA: Personnel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisberg, R. W. (1999). Creativity and knowledge: A challenge to theories. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Handbook of creativity(pp. 226–250). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, L. F. & Sternberg, R. J. (2006).The nature of intellectual styles. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer Science + Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sternberg, R.J. (2009). Domain-Generality Versus Domain-Specificity of Creativity. In: Meusburger, P., Funke, J., Wunder, E. (eds) Milieus of Creativity. Knowledge and Space, vol 2. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-9877-2_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics