Advertisement

From Population to Organization Thinking

  • David Lane
  • Robert Maxfield
  • Dwight Read
  • Sander van der Leeuw
Part of the Methodos Series book series (METH, volume 7)

Our species is still very young by biological time scales, and it is too early to know if we represent the cutting edge of a biological success story, like cockroaches or dinosaurs, or a brilliant but ultimately failed and short-lived experiment in niche construction and destruction. In the mere 200,000 or so years of Homo sapiens’ story, and in particular in the approximately 50,000 years since we began to accrue the accoutrements of culture like language, art and multi-component artifacts, members of our species have populated a vast extent of the earth’s surface and exploited for our own purposes an ever-increasing share of the planet’s biologically utilizable solar energy.

Keywords

Niche Construction Artifact Type Kennel Club Cocker Spaniel Breed Standard 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Agar, M. (1994). Language shock: Understanding the culture of conversation. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
  2. Beinhocker, E. (2006). The origin of wealth: Evolution, complexity, and the radical remaking of economics. Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.Google Scholar
  3. Caddy, J. (1995). Cocker spaniels today: Book of the breed.Lydney, UK: Ringpress Books.Google Scholar
  4. Croft, W. (2001). Explaining language change: An evolutionary approach. Harlow, UK: Longman Linguistics Library.Google Scholar
  5. Desmond, A., & Moore, J. (1991). Darwin: The life of a tormented evolutionist. New York: Warner Books.Google Scholar
  6. Edelman, G. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  7. Holland, J., Holyoak, K., Nisbet, R., & Thagard, P. (1989). Induction: Processes of learning, inference and discovery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  8. Jerne, N. (1967). Antibodies and learning: Selection versus instruction. In: G. Quarton, T. Melnechuk, & F. Schmidt (Eds.), The neurosciences: A study program (pp. 200–205). New York: Rockefeller University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Lane, D., Malerba, F., Maxfield, R., & Orsenigo, L. (1996). Choice and action. Journal of Evolutionary Economics, 6, 43–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Lane, D., & Maxfield, R. (1997). Foresight, complexity and strategy. In: W. B. Arthur, S. Durlauf, & D. Lane (Eds.), Economy as a complex, evolving system II (pp. 169–198). Redwood City, CA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  11. Lane, D., & Maxfield, R. (2005). Ontological uncertainty and innovation. Journal of Evolutionary Economics, 15, 3–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Lehmann-Haupt, H. (1950). Peter Schoeffer of Gernsheim and Mainz. Rochester, NY: Leo Hart.Google Scholar
  13. Mayr, E. (1959). Darwin and the evolutionary theory in biology. In: B. Meggers, Evolution and anthropology: A centennial appraisal (pp. 1–10). Washington, DC: Anthropological Society of Washington.Google Scholar
  14. Mayr, E. (1991). One long argument: Charles Darwin and the genesis of modern evolutionary thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • David Lane
    • 1
  • Robert Maxfield
  • Dwight Read
  • Sander van der Leeuw
  1. 1.Department of Social, Cognitive and Quantitative SciencesUniversity of Modena and Reggio EmiliaReggio EmiliaItaly

Personalised recommendations