Skip to main content

Connections in Action – Bridging Implicit and Explicit Domains

  • Chapter
A Legacy for Living Systems

Part of the book series: Biosemiotics ((BSEM,volume 2))

Implicit and explicit learning and knowledge processes differ with respect to acquisition as well as expression of knowledge. For instance implicit knowledge improves correlative to extended practise, is normally unknown to the subject and is closed to verbal reports.

On the contrary, explicit knowledge works proportionately well on single exposures to stimuli, is accessible to awareness and is preferentially recoverable as propositional, rational statements, conceptualisations and the like. Standard view is that implicit and explicit learning are separately engaged as they attend to different tasks and seem to occupy distinct neural regions. Here, we report on results obtained in functional imaging studies, which suggest a revision of this view. The results imply a positive effect of extended naturalistic practise on processes responsible for categorising items in an object recognition task – knowledge traditionally thought of as explicit.

The results therefore provide evidence of how implicit learning might influence explicit knowledge at the neural level.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 229.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Bateson, G. (1979). Mind and Nature: A Necessary Unity. Toronto: Bantam Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayley, P. J. and Squire, L. R. (2002). “Medial temporal lobe amnesia: Gradual acquisition of factual information by nondeclarative memory.” Journal of Neuroscience 22(13): 5741–5748.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J. (2001). “Ideomotor apraxia: A call to action.” Neurocase(7): 445–458.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J. and Saffran, E. M. (2002). “Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and nonapraxic subjects.” Brain and Language 18: 179–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F. and Carew, T. G. (1997). “The role of semantic memory in object use.” Cognitive Neuropsychology 14: 219–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., Veramonti, T. and Schwartz, M. F. (2000). “Function and manipulation tool knowledge in apraxia: Knowing ‘what for’ but not ‘how’.” Neurocase (6): 83–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chao, L. L. and Martin, A. (2000). “Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stream.” NeuroImage 12: 478–484.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, H., Daly, E., Phillips, M., Brammer, M., Bullmore, E., Williams, S., Van Amelsvoort, T., Robertson, D., David, A. and Murphy, D. (2000). “Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study.” Human Brain Mapping 9: 93–105.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1996). Kinds of Minds. London, Weidenfeld & Nicholson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gainotti, G. (2002). The relationships between anatomical and cognitive locus of lesion in category-specific disorders. Category Specificity in Brain and Mind. G.W. Humphreys and E.M.E. Forde. Hove, Psychology Press: 403–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C. and Rizzolatti, G. (2004). “A unifying view of the basis of social cognition.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(9): 396–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerlach, G., Law, I., Gade, A. and Paulson, O. B. (2000). “Categorization and category effects in normal object recognition: A PET study.” Neuropsychologia 38: 1693–1703.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gerlach, G., Law, I., Gade, A. and Paulson, O. B. (2002a). “The role of action knowledge in the comprehension of artefacts–A PET study.” NeuroImage 7: 232–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerlach, G., Law, I and Paulson, O. B. (2002b). “When action turns into words. Activation of motor-based knowledge during categorization of manipulable objects.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14(8): 1230–1239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, K. and Ingold, T. Eds. (1993). Tools, Language and Cognition in Human Evolution. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafton, S. T., Fadiga, L., Arbib, M. A. and Rizzolatti, G. (1997). “Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools.” NeuroImage 6: 231–236.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I. and Pulvermüller, F. (2004). “Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex.” Neuron 41: 301–307.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., Maher, L. M., Greenwald, M. L. and Rothi, L. J. G. (1997). “Conceptual apraxia from lateralized lesions.” Neurology 49: 457–464.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W. and Riddoch, M. J. (1999). Impaired development of semantic memory: Separating semantic from structural knowledge and diagnosing a role for action in establishing stored memories for objects. Neurocase 5: 519–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsner, K., Speelman, C., Maybery, M., O’Brien-Malone, A., Anderson, M. and MacLeod, C. Eds. (1998). Implicit and Explicit Mental Processes. Mahwah, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. (1998). Implicit perception: Perceptual processing without awareness. Implicit and Explicit Mental Processes. K. Kirsner, C. Speelman, M. Maybery et al. Mahwah, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers: 57–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Ungerleider, L. G. and Haxby, J. V. (1996). “Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge.” Nature 379: 649–652.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moreaud, O., Charnallet, A. and Pellat, J. (1998). “Identification without manipulation: A study of the relations between object use and semantic memory.” Neuropsychologia 36: 1295–1301.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, W. and Davidson, I. (1996). Human Evolution, Language and Mind: A Psychological and Archaeological Inquiry. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, C. J. and Friston, K. J. (1999). “Scanning patients with tasks they can perform.” Human Brain Mapping 8: 102–108.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Price, C. J., Mummery, C. J., Moore, C. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J. and Friston, K. J. (1999). “Delineating necessary and sufficient neural systems with functional imaging studies of neuropsychological patients.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11: 371–382.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermuller, F., Harle, M. and Hummel, F. (2001). “Walking or talking? Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of action verb processing.” Behavioral and Neurophysiological Correlates of Action Verb Processing 78: 143–168.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Raymer, A. M. and Ochipa, C. (1997). Conceptual praxis. Apraxia: The Neuropsychology of Action. L.J.G. Rothi and K.M. Heilman. Hove, Psychology Press: 51–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1992). “The cognitive unconscious: An evolutionary perspective.” Consciousness and Cognition 1: 93–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge. Oxford, Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G. and Fadiga, L. (1998). “Grasping objects and grasping action meanings: the dual role of monkey rostroventral premotor cortex (area F5).” Novartis Foundation Symposium 218: 81–95.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rosci, C., Chiesa, V., Laiacona, M. and Capitani, E. (2003). “Apraxia is not associated to a disproportionate naming impairment for manipulable objects.” Brain and Cognition (53): 412–415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rugg, M. D., Mark, R. E., Walla, P., Schloerscheidt, A. M., Birch, C. S. and Allan, K.,(1998). “Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory.” Nature 392(April): 595–598.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schilhab, T. S. S. (2007). “Knowledge for real–on implicit and explicit representations and education.” Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research 51(3): 223–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schilhab, T. S. S. and Gerlach, C. (in print). Learning Bodies. T. Moser, T. Schilhab and M. Juelskjær. The Danish School of Education Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G. and Vanderwart, M. (1980). “A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 6: 174–215.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Speelman, C. and Maybery, M. (1998). Automaticity and skill acquisition. Implicit and Explicit Mental Processes. K. Kirsner, C. Speelman, M. Maybery et al. Mahwah, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers: 79–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R. and Zola-Morgan, S. (1988). “Memory: brain systems and behavior.” Trends in Neuro Science 11(4): 170–175.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stadler, M. A. and Frensch, P. A. Eds. (1998). Handbook of Implicit Learning. Thousand Oaks, Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, G. Ed. (1996). Implicit Cognition. Oxford, Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Schie, H. T., Mars, R. B., Coles, M. G. H. and Bekkering, H. (2004). “Modulation of activity in medial frontal and motor cortices during error observation.” Nature Neuroscience 7(5): 549–554.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wackerhausen, S. (1997). “The scholastic paradigm and apprenticeship.” Nordisk Pedagogik 17(3): 195–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynne, C. D. L. (1998). A natural history of explicit learning and memory. Implicit and Explicit Mental Processes. K. Kirsner, C. Speelman, M. Maybery et al. Mahwah, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2008 Springer Science + Business Media B.V

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Schilhab, T.S.S., Gerlach, C. (2008). Connections in Action – Bridging Implicit and Explicit Domains. In: Hoffmeyer, J. (eds) A Legacy for Living Systems. Biosemiotics, vol 2. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-6706-8_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics