Skip to main content

Collapsing the Wave Function of Meaning: The Epistemological Matrix of Talk-in-Interaction

  • Chapter
A Legacy for Living Systems

Part of the book series: Biosemiotics ((BSEM,volume 2))

Devoted to an explication of how interacting agents mutually and micro-temporally provide for each other the grounds for immediate next action in the seemingly transparent give-and-take of ordinary conversation, empirical findings from the disciplines of Interaction Analysis suggest that “language” as it is actually realized in naturally occurring, everyday talk-in-interaction, may derive its semiotic efficacy more from the active co-participation of situated speakers in creating contexts of relevancy, constraint and possibility for each other’s immediate next re-shaping of the cybernetic surround than it does from the computational recombination of referential tokens within the bounds of some predetermined, category-structuring syntax.

The twin purposes of this article are to: (1) to serve as an introduction to some of the basic principles, methodologies and research data of Interaction Analysis, and (2) to attempt to situate such research and its findings within the broader study of meaning-making among living agents that is the goal of a Gregory Batesoninspired biosemiotics. Here I hope to show how the former can well illuminate latter’s efforts to explicate the principles whereby not only our human social worlds – but our very biological world itself – comes into being not as a “pre-given” in the furniture of the universe, but as a locally organized, massively co-constructed, participant-fashioned accomplishment in that universe instead.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 229.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Atkinson, J. M., & Drew, P. (1979). Order in Court: The Organization of Verbal Interaction in Judicial Settings. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). From attention-goal psychology to belief-desire psychology: The development of a theory of mind and its dysfunction. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1979). Mind and Nature: A Necessary Unity. New York: E.P. Dutton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (2000). Steps to an Ecology of Mind; Collected Essays in Anthropology, Psychiatry, Evolution, and Epistemology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergman, A., & Fahey, M. F. (1999). Ours, Yours, Mine: Mutuality and the Emergence of the Separate Self. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1968). Language and Mind. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and Representations. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D. (1967). A segmentation of behavior. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 5(3), 221–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Condon, W. S., & Osgton, W. D. (1971). Speech and body motion synchrony of the speaker-hearer. In D. H. Horton & J. J. Jenkins (Eds.), The Perception of Language, New York: Academic. pp. 150–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Condon, W., & Sander, L. (1974). Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: Interactional participation and language acquisition. Science, 183, 99–101.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. W. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language and the Brain New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Casper, A., & Fifer W. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborn’s prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A., & Goodwin, C. (1992). Rethinking Context: Language as a n Interactive Phenomenon. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Favareau, D. (2001). Constructing representema: On the neurosemiotics of self and vision. Semiotics, Evolution, Energy and Development Journal, 2(4), 3–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Favareau, D. (2004). A Synthesis of Biosemiotics and Interaction Analysis for the Investigation of Experience as a Natural Category. Doctoral Dissertation, UCLA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C., & Yirmira, N. (1999). Mother–infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 17, 347–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A., & Branco, A. (1997). Metacommunication as a source of indeterminism in relationship development. In Fogel, A., Lyra, M. and Valsiner, J. (Eds.), Dynamics and Indeterminism in Developmental and Social Processes. Mahweh, NJ: Erlbaum (pp. 65–92).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, C. E., Fox, B. A., & Thompson, S. A. (2002). The Language of Turn and Sequence. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1996). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind reading. Brain, 119, 543–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1950). The Perception of the Visual World. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in Public Places; Notes on the Social Organization of Gatherings. New York: Free Press of Glencoe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1979). The interactive construction of a sentence in natural conversation. In G. Psathas (Ed.), Everyday Language: Studies in Ethnomethodology (pp. 97–121). New York: Irvington Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1980). Re-starts, pauses, and the achievement of a state of mutual gaze at turn-beginning. Sociological Inquiry, 50(3–4), 272–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1981). Conversational Organization: Interaction Between Speakers and Hearers. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1986). Gesture as a resource for the organization of mutual orientation. Semiotica, 62(1/2), 29–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1995a). Co-constructing meaning in conversations with an aphasic man. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 28(3), 233–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1995b). Seeing in depth. Social Studies of Science, 25: 237–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1997). The blackness of black: Color categories as situated practice. In B. Burge (Ed.), Discourse, Tools and Reasoning: Essays on Situated Cognition (pp. 111–140). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2000a). Action and embodiment within situated human interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 1489–1522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2000b). Gesture, aphasia and interaction. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and Gesture (pp. 84–98). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2000c). Pointing and the collaborative construction of meaning in aphasia. Texas Linguistic Forum (Proceedings of the seventh annual Symposium About Language and Society-Austin SALSA), 43, 67–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2002). Conversational frameworks for the accomplishment of meaning in aphasia. In C. Goodwin (Ed.), Situating Language Impairments within Conversation. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C., & Goodwin, M. H. (1987). Concurrent operations on talk: Notes on the interactive organization of assessments. IPrA Papers in Pragmatics, 1(1), 1–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C., & Goodwin, M. H. (1992). Assessments and the construction of context. In A. Duranti & C. Goodwin (Eds.), Rethinking Context: Language as an Interactive Phenomenon (pp. 147–190). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, M. H. (1990). He-Said-She-Said: Talk as Social Organization among Black Children. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grassé, P. P. (1959). La reconstruction du nid et les coordinations inter-individuelles chez Belicositermes natalensis et Cubitermes sp. La théorie de la Stigmergie. Insectes Sociaux 6, 41–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenough, W., Black, J., and Wallace, C. (1987). Experience and brain development. Child Development 58, 539–559.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heritage, J. (1984). Garfinkel and Ethnomethodology. Cambridge: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (1996). Signs of Meaning in the Universe. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jefferson, G. (1984). Notes on some orderliness of overlap onset. In V. D’Urso & P. Leonardi (Eds.), Discourse Analysis and Natural Rhetorics (pp. 11–38). Padova: Cleup Editore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jefferson, G. (1987). On exposed and embedded correction in conversation. In G. Button & J.Lee (Eds.), Talk and Social Organization (pp. 86–100). Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M. (2005) Understanding Others: Imitation, Language, Empathy. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on Imitation: From Cognitive Neuroscience to Social Science. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kauffman, S. A. (1995). At Home in the Universe: The Search for Laws of Self-Organization and Complexity. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (1990). Conducting Interaction: Patterns of Behavior in Focused Encounters. Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1993). We Have Never Been Modern. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1999). Pandora’s Hope: Essays on the Reality of Science Studies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B., & Woolgar, S. (1979). Laboratory Life: The Social Construction of Scientific Facts. Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, G. (1996). On the place of linguistic resources in the organization of talk-in interaction: “Second Person” reference in multi-party conversation. Pragmatics, 6(3), 281–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, G. (1991). On the syntax of sentences-in-progress. Language in Society, 20, 441–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lester, B., Hoffman, J. and Brazelton, T. (1985). The rhythmic structure of mother–infant interaction in term and preterm infants. Child Development, 56, 15–27.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewontin, R. C. (1983). The organism as subject and object of evolution. Scientia 188, 65–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewontin, R. C. (2000). The Triple Helix: Gene, Organism, and Environment. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merritt, M. (1976) On questions following questions in service encounters. Language in Society 5.3, pp. 315–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morita. E. (2005). Negotiation of Contingent Talk: The Japanese Interactional Particles ‘Ne’ and ‘Sa.’ Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. S. (2001). The Dependent Gene: The Fallacy of Nature/Nurture. New York: Times Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., Gonzales, P., & Jacoby, S. (1996). “When I come down, I’m in a domain state:” Grammar and graphic representation in the interpretive activity of physicists. In E. Ochs, E. Schegloff & S. Thompson (Eds.), Interaction and Grammar (pp. 328–369). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., Schegloff, E. A., & Thompson, S. A. (1996). Interaction and Grammar. Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct (1st Ed.). New York: W. Morrow and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, A. (1984). Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: Some features of preferred/dispreferred turn shapes. In J. Heritage (Ed.), Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis (pp. 57–101). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prevignano, C. & Thibault, P. (2003). Discussing Conversational Analysis: The Work of Emmanuel A. Schegloff. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rüting, T. (2004) Jakob von Uexküll: Theoretical Biology, Biocybernetics and Biosemiotics. Online at: www.math.uni-hamburg.de/home/rueting/UexECMTB.doc.

  • Sacks, H. (1995). Lectures on Conversation. In G. Jefferson & E. Schegloff (Eds.), Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50, 696–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1976). On some questions and ambiguities in conversation. In W. U. Dressler (Ed.), Current Trends in Textlinguistics (pp. 81–102). New York: Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1980). Preliminaries to preliminaries: ‘Can I ask you a question.’ Sociological Inquiry, 50, 104–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1982). Discourse as an interactional achievement: Some uses of ‘uh huh’ and other things that come between sentences. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Georgetown University Roundtable on Languages and Linguistics (pp. 71–93). Washington DC: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1986). The routine as achievement. Human Studies, 9, 111–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1988). On an actual virtual servo-mechanism for guessing bad news: A single case conjecture. Social Problems, 35(4), 442–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1990). On the organization of sequences as a source of “coherence” in talk-in-interaction. In B. Dorval (Ed.), Conversational Organization and its Development (pp. 51–77). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1991). Conversation analysis and socially shared cognition. In S. D. Teasley (Ed.), Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition (pp. 150–171). Washington: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1992). Repair after next turn: The last structurally provided defense of intersubjectivity in conversation. American Journal of Sociology, 97(5), 1295–1345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1995a). Sequence Organization. Unpublished Manuscript, University of California, Los Angeles.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E.A. (1995b). Introduction. In G Jefferson & E. Schegloff (Eds.), Lectures on Conversation. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1997). Issues of relevance for discourse analysis: Contingency in action, interaction and co-participant context. In E. H. Hovy & D. Scott (Eds.), Computational and Conversational Discourse: Burning Issues – An Interdisciplinary Account (pp. 3–38). Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1998). Body torque. Social Research, 65(3), 535–596.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (2000). Overlapping talk and the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language in Society, 29, 1–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A., Jefferson, G., & Sacks, H. (1977). The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language, 53, 361–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A., & Sacks, H. (1973). Opening up closings. Semiotica, 8, 289–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schore, A. (2001). Effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect regulation and infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22(1–2), 7–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schumann, J., Favareau, D., Goodwin, C., Lee, N., Mikesell, L., Tao, H., Veronique, D. and Wray, A. (2006). Language evolution: What evolved? Marges Linguistiques 11, 167–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverstein, M. (2003). Indexical order and the dialectics of sociolinguistic life. Language and Communication, 23(3–4), 193–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solé, R. V., & Goodwin, B. C. (2001). Signs of Life: How Complexity Pervades Biology. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strawson, P.F. (1971). Philosophic Logic. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streeck, J. (1996). How to do things with things. Human Studies, 19, 365–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teraski, A. (1976). Pre-Announcement Sequences in Conversation. Social Science Working Paper 99. Irvine, CA: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Theraulaz, G., Bonabeau, E. and Deneubourg, J.L. (1998). The origin of nest complexity in social insects. Complexity 3.6, 15–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1987). Sharing makes sense: Intersubjectivie motor control in infants. In H. T. A. Whiting (Ed.), Motor Development in Children. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1993). The self born in intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The Perceived Self: Ecological and Interpersonal Resources of Self-Knowledge (pp. 121–173). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (2001). Intrinsic motives for comparionship in understanding: Their origin, development and significance for infant mental health. International Journal of Infant Mental Health, 22(1–2), 95–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turkewitz, G., & Devenny, D. A. (1993). Developmental Time and Timing. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wisdom, J. (1952 [1968]). Other Minds. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical Investigations. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1984). Culture and Value. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2008 Springer Science + Business Media B.V

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Favareau, D. (2008). Collapsing the Wave Function of Meaning: The Epistemological Matrix of Talk-in-Interaction. In: Hoffmeyer, J. (eds) A Legacy for Living Systems. Biosemiotics, vol 2. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-6706-8_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics