Skip to main content

The Child’s and the Practical View of Space

  • Chapter
  • 16 Accesses

Part of the book series: Critical Human Geography ((CHG))

Abstract

In this chapter we are considering the core of the unsophisticated-fused pattern of thought — the evolution of the child’s view to that of the practical view of the untutored adult. In these views we do not find a high degree of conceptual separation between subjective and objective and space and substance. What conceptual separation develops depends on the use of symbols. In general, we can say that the growth and development of individuals and species is characterised by several interrelated processes: the increasing complexity of the organism, its increasingly complex assessment of its environment, and the increasingly complex interrelationships between the environment and the organism.1 In the earlier stages of development the organism’s parts are not very specialised. There are few centres of organisation and control. The activities of the organism are not well integrated hierarchically and a few or even one action may often involve the entire body, so that the organism cannot attend to more than a limited number of things at a time. In such circumstances, an organism’s actions are said to be global and syncretic.2 In later stages the actions become more discrete, differentiated, articulated and hierarchically integrated.3

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Notes and References

  1. Heinz Werner, Comparative Psychology of Mental Development (New York: International Universities Press, 1973) pp. 40–56d.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Jean Piaget, The Child’s Conception of the World (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1971) pp. 169–252;

    Google Scholar 

  3. Bruno Bettelheim, The Uses of Enchantment (New York: Random House, 1977) pp. 45–7.

    Google Scholar 

  4. William Stern, Psychology of Early Childhood (German edition, 1930) p. 46.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Jonas Langer, Theories of Development (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1969) pp. 129–31.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Rudolf Arnheim, Visual Thinking (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1974)

    Google Scholar 

  7. H. Price, Thinking and Experience (Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1953).

    Google Scholar 

  8. James Gibson, The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems (Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1966).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Yi-Fu Tuan, Topophilia (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1974) p. 27.

    Google Scholar 

  10. David Lowenthal’s discussion, ‘Geography, Experience and Imagination: Toward a Geographical Epistemology’, Annals, Association of American Geographers, vol. 51 (1961) pp. 241–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Mircea Eliade, Australian Religions (Ithaca: Cornell University Press, 1973) p. 42.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Emile Durkheim and Marcel Mauss, Primitive Classification (University of Chicago Press, 1963).

    Google Scholar 

  13. O. Ortmann, ‘Theories of Synaesthesia in the Light of a Case of Colour Hearing’, Human Biology, vol. 5 (1933) pp. 155–211.

    Google Scholar 

  14. L. Riggs and T. Karwoski, ‘Synaesthesia’, British Journal of Psychology, vol. 25 (1934) pp. 29–41.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Arthur Symons, ‘The Opium-Smoker’, Poems by Arthur Symons (New York: John Lane, 1912) vol. 1, p. 3.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Susanne Langer, Mind (Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins Press, 1967) vol. 1, pp. 183–5.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Stephen Ullmann, The Principles of Semantics (Glasgow: Jackson, 1957) p. 280.

    Google Scholar 

  18. S. Newhall, ‘Warmth and Coolness of Colours’, Psychological Record, vol. 4 (1941) pp. 198–212.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Deborah Sharpe, The Psychology of Color Design (Chicago: Nelson-Hall, 1974).

    Google Scholar 

  20. Heinz Werner and Bernard Kaplan, Symbol Formation (New York: Wiley, 1963) p. 208.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Stephen Ullmann, Semantics (New York: Barnes and Noble, 1964) pp. 80–115.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Alexander Pope, An Essay on Criticisms, in vol. 1, The Complete Poetical Works of Alexander Pope (Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1903).

    Google Scholar 

  23. Ernst Cassirer, Philosophy of Symbolic Forms (New Haven: Yale University, 1955) vol. 1; Werner and Kaplan, Symbol Formation, and Willbur Urban, Language and Reality (New York: Macmillan, 1939).

    Google Scholar 

  24. Otto Jespersen, Language: Its Nature, Development and Origin (London: George Allen and Unwin, 1922) p. 405.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Herbert Clark, ‘Space, Time, Semantics, and the Child’, in Timothy Moore (ed.), Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language (New York: Academic Press, 1973) pp. 27–63.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Jean Piaget, The Language and Thought of the Child (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1959) 3rd edition, p. 13.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Ronald Longacker, Language and its Structure (New York: Harcourt Brace and World, 1968) p. 18.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Edward Sapir, ‘Study in Phonetic Symbolism’, Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 12 (1929) pp. 225–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Copyright information

© 1980 Robert David Sack

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sack, R.D. (1980). The Child’s and the Practical View of Space. In: Conceptions of Space in Social Thought. Critical Human Geography. Palgrave, London. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-16433-2_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics