Skip to main content

The Effects of Trait-anxiety on Anagram Solution Times

  • Chapter
Book cover New Concepts in Anxiety
  • 71 Accesses

Abstract

As anyone who has struggled in vain with the clues of a cryptic crossword will appreciate, the ease and speed with which we can solve anagrams depends largely on practice. However, other factors are important. It is generally easier to solve anagrams of shorter words than longer ones (Kaplan and Carvellas, 1968). Solution times are also faster for words with a higher frequency of usage (Mayzner and Tresselt, 1958). Perhaps more interestingly, Hunter (1956) showed that if subjects are instructed that all of the anagram solutions are taken from a particular semantic category (e.g. animals) then the mean solution time is faster than for naive controls. Boddy (1968) demonstrated that a task-induced set proves just as effective at increasing solution speed as the instructional set used by Hunter. He presented subjects with groups of anagrams, the solutions of which were drawn from the same category. After sucessfully solving two or three anagrams, the subjects formed the appropriate mental set (e.g. all solutions are animal words) and solved the remaining anagrams more quickly.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Boddy, J. (1968). Task-induced set and anagram solution. Psych. Reports, 23, 1162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourne, L. E., Ekstrand, B. R. and Dominowski, R. L. (1971). The Psychology of Thinking. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalgleish, T. and Watts, F. N. (1990). Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression. Clin. Psych. Reviews, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, C. P. (1966). Problem solving with a verbal response hierarchy. Psychonomic Science, 4, 147–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., MacLeod, C. and Mathews, A. (1987). Cognitive functioning and anxiety. Psychological Research, 49, 189–95.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, I. M. L. (1956). The influence of set on problem solving. Br. J. Psychol., 57, 63–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • John, C. H. (1988). Emotionality ratings and free-association norms of 240 emotional and non-emotional words. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 49–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, I. T. and Carvellas, T. (1968). Effect of word length on anagram solution time. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav., 7, 201–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., Richards, A. and Eysenck, M. W. (1989). Interpretation of homophones related to threat in anxiety states. J. Abnormal Psych., 98, 31–4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mayzner, M. S. and Tresselt, M. E. (1958). Anagram solution times: a function of letter order and word frequency. J. Exp. Psych., 56, 376–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 1991 Macmillan Publishers Limited

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Dalgleish, T. (1991). The Effects of Trait-anxiety on Anagram Solution Times. In: Briley, M., File, S.E. (eds) New Concepts in Anxiety. Palgrave, London. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-11847-2_34

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-11847-2_34

  • Publisher Name: Palgrave, London

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-349-11849-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-349-11847-2

  • eBook Packages: EngineeringEngineering (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics