Advertisement

Linguistic Theory, Linguistic Diversity and Whorfian Economics

  • Nigel Fabb

Abstract

I begin this chapter by illustrating what theoretical linguists do and why generative linguistics in particular has argued that linguistic data is more abstract and more complex than at first appears. I then illustrate language diversity by comparing aspects of two languages, English and Ma’di, an East African language. Sections 1.2 and 1.3 focus on specific aspects of linguistic data, which are chosen to illustrate general points about linguistic theory and linguistic diversity. In Section 1.4, I briefly note several different theories of linguistic diversity which explore why languages vary and whether variation is limited by internal (psychological) or external (cultural) factors. In Section 1.5, I examine the ‘Whorfian’ hypothesis that linguistic form has a causal relation with thought and behaviour, summarize some of the relevant psycholinguistic work and then examine some articles by economists which claim causal relations between linguistic and social variation. Based in part on what I have said about linguistic theory and linguistic diversity, and in particular the abstractness of linguistic data, I will argue that the economists’ claims cannot be sustained. In the final part of the chapter I discuss some ways in which stylistic variation within a language might affect thought.

Keywords

Noun Phrase Linguistic Diversity Linguistic Theory Constituent Structure Linguistic Form 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. T. Au (1983) ‘Chinese and English Counterfactuals: The Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis Revisited’, Cognition, 15, 155–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. B. Bassetti (2014) ‘Is Grammatical Gender Considered Arbitrary or Semantically Motivated? Evidence from Young Adult Monolinguals, Second Language Learners, and Early Bilinguals’, British Journal of Psychology, 105, 273–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. A. Bender, S. Beller and G. Bennardo (2010) ‘Temporal Frames of Reference: Conceptual Analysis and Empirical Evidence from German, English, Mandarin Chinese, and Tongan’, Journal of Cognition and Culture, 10, 283–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. B. Bernstein (1971) Class, Codes and Control. Volume 1: Theoretical Studies towards a Sociology of Language (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. R. Berwick, P. Pietroski, B. Yankama and N. Chomsky (2011) ‘Poverty of the Stimulus Revisited’, Cognitive Science, 1–36.Google Scholar
  6. H. den Besten (1983) ‘On the Interaction of Root Transformations and Lexical Deletive Rules’, revised version of a paper in Studies in West Germanic Syntax, Dissertatie Tilburg, 1989, 20, 14–100.Google Scholar
  7. T. Biberauer, A. Holmberg and I. Roberts (2007) ‘Disharmonic Word-order Systems and the Final-over-Final-Constraint (FOFC)’ In A. Bisetto and F. Barbieri (eds) Proceedings of XXXIII Incontro di Grammatica Generativa. Available at http://amsacta.cib.unibo.it/archive/00002397/01/PROCEEDINGS_IGG.pdf
  8. M. Blackings and N. Fabb (2003) A Grammar of Ma’di (Berlin: Mouton).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. A. Bloom (1981) The Linguistic Shaping of Thought: A Study in the Impact of Language on Thinking in China and the West (Hillsdale: Erlbaum Associates).Google Scholar
  10. L. Boroditsky (2001) ‘Does Language Shape Thought? Mandarin and English Speakers’ Conceptions of Time’, Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. L. Boroditsky, L Schmidt and W. Phillips (2003) ‘Sex, Syntax, and Semantics’ In D. Gentner and S. Goldin-Meadow (eds) Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Cognition (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).Google Scholar
  12. B. Boutonnet, P. Athanasopoulos and G. Thierry (2012) ‘Unconscious Effects of Grammatical Gender during Object Categorisation’, Brain Research, 1479, 72–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. J.-Y. Chen (2007) ‘Do Chinese and English Speakers Think about Time Differently? Failure of Replicating Boroditsky (2001)’, Cognition, 104, 427–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. K. Chen (2013) ‘The Effect of Language on Economic Behavior: Evidence from Savings Rates, Health Behaviors, and Retirement Assets’, The American Economic Review, 103, 690–731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. N. Chomsky (1957) Syntactic Structures (The Hague: Mouton).Google Scholar
  16. N. Chomsky (1965) Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).Google Scholar
  17. N. Chomsky (1981) Lectures on Government and Binding (Dordrecht: Foris).Google Scholar
  18. N. Chomsky (1995) The Minimalist Program (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).Google Scholar
  19. N. Chomsky and M. Halle (1968) The Sound Pattern of English (New York: Harper & Row).Google Scholar
  20. G. Cinque (2013) ‘Cognition, Universal Grammar, and Typological Generalizations’, Lingua, 130, 50–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. P. Cole, G. Hermon and L-M. Sung (1990) ‘Principles and Parameters of Long-Distance Reflexives’, Linguistic Inquiry, 21, 1–22.Google Scholar
  22. G. Corbett (1991) Gender (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. G. Corbett (2013) ‘“30 Number of Genders”, “31 Sex-based and Non-sex-based Gender Systems”, “32 Systems of Gender Assignment”’ In M. Dryer and M. Haspelmath (eds) The World Atlas of Language Structures Online (Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology).Google Scholar
  24. R. Cubelli, D. Paolieri, L. Lotto and R. Job (2011) ‘The Effect of Grammatical Gender on Object Categorization’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 449–460.Google Scholar
  25. Ö. Dahl and V. Velupillai (2013) ‘Tense and Aspect’ In Matthew S. Dryer and M. Haspelmath (eds) The World Atlas of Language Structures Online (Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology). Available online at http://wals.info/chapter/s7 (Accessed on 2014-06-16).Google Scholar
  26. D. Dediu and D. R. Ladd (2007) ‘Linguistic Tone Is Related to the Population Frequency of the Adaptive Haplogroups of Two Brain Size Genes, ASPM and Microcephalin’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 10944–10949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. M. Dryer (2013) ‘144 Position of Negative Morpheme with Respect to Subject, Object, and Verb’ In M. Dryer and M. Haspelmath (eds) The World Atlas of Language Structures Online (Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology).Google Scholar
  28. N. Evans and S. Levinson (2009) ‘The Myth of Language Universals: Language Diversity and its Importance for Cognitive Science’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 429–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. V. Gay, E. Santacreu-Vasut and A. Shoham (2013) ‘The Grammatical Origins of Gender Roles’, BEHL Working Paper.Google Scholar
  30. L. Gleitman and A. Papafragou (2005) ‘Language and Thought’ In K. Holyoak and R. Morrison (eds) Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning, 633–662 (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).Google Scholar
  31. L. Haegeman and T. Ihsane (2001) ‘Adult Null Subjects in the Non-pro-drop Languages: Two Diary Dialects’, Language Acquisition, 9, 329–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. H. Harley and E. Ritter (2002) ‘Person and Number in Pronouns: A Feature-Geometric Analysis’, Language, 78, 482–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. L. Hasher, D. Goldstein and T. Toppino (1977) ‘Frequency and the Conference of Referential Validity’, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 107–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. M. Haspelmath (2013) ‘34 Occurrence of Nominal Plurality’ In M. Dryer and M. Haspelmath (eds) The World Atlas of Language Structures Online (Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology).Google Scholar
  35. J. Hill and B. Mannheim (1992) ‘Language and World View’, Annual Review of Anthropology, 21, 381–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. C-T. J. Huang (1989) ‘Pro Drop in Chinese: A Generalized Control Approach’ In O. Jaeggli and K. Safir (eds) The Null Subject Parameter, 185–214 (Dordrecht: D. Reidel).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. M. Imai, L. Schalk, H. Saalbach and H. Okada (2014) ‘All Giraffes Have Female-Specific Properties: Influence of Grammatical Gender on Deductive Reasoning about Sex-Specific Properties in German Speakers’, Cognitive Science, 38, 514–536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. D. January and E. Kako (2007) ‘Re-evaluating Evidence for Linguistic Relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001)’, Cognition, 104, 417–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. E. Kashima and Y. Kashima (1998) ‘Culture and Language: The Case of Cultural Dimensions and Personal Pronoun Use’, Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 461–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. E. Kashima and Y. Kashima (2005) ‘Erratum to Kashima and Kashima (1998) and Reiteration’, Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 396–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. S. Levinson (1996) ‘Frames of Reference and Molyneux’s Question: Crosslinguistic Evidence’ In P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel and M. Garrett (eds) Language and Space, 109–169 (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).Google Scholar
  42. S. Levinson (1997) ‘From Outer to Inner Space: Linguistic Categories and Non-linguistic Thinking’ In J. Nuyts and E. Pederson (eds) Language and Conceptualization, 13–45 (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. S. Levinson (2003) Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. P. Li, L. Abarbanell, L. Gleitman and A. Papafragou (2011) ‘Spatial Reasoning in Tenejapan Mayans’, Cognition, 120, 33–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. P. Li and L. Gleitman (2002) ‘Turning the Tables: Language and Spatial Reasoning’, Cognition, 83, 265–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. A. Licht, C. Goldschmidt and S. Schwartz (2007) ‘Culture Rules: The Foundations of the Rule of Law and other Norms of Governance’, Journal of Comparative Economics, 35, 659–688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. J. Lucy (1997) ‘Linguistic Relativity’, Annual Review of Anthropology, 26, 291–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. I. Maddieson (2013) ‘1 Consonant Inventories’, ‘2 Vowel Quality Inventories’, ‘7 Glottalized Consonants’, ‘12 Syllable Structure’, ‘13 Tone’, ‘19 Presence of Uncommon Consonants’ In M. Dryer and M. Haspelmath (eds) The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. (Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology).Google Scholar
  49. A. Mavisakalyan (2011) ‘Gender in Language and Gender in Employment’, ANUCBE School of Economics Working Paper.Google Scholar
  50. M. McGlone and J. Tofighbakhsh (2000) ‘Birds of a Feather Flock Conjointly (?): Rhyme as Reason in Aphorisms’, Psychological Science, 11, 424–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. J. Nichols (2012) ‘Monogenesis or Polygenesis: A Single Ancestral Language for All Humanity?’ In M. Tallerman and K. Gibson (eds) The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution (Oxford: Oxford University Press).Google Scholar
  52. J. Prewitt-Freilino, T. Caswell and E. Laakso (2012) ‘The Gendering of Language: A Comparison of Gender Equality in Countries with Gendered, Natural Gender, and Genderless Languages’, Sex Roles, 66, 268–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. L. Rizzi (2013) ‘Introduction: Core Computational Principles in Natural Language Syntax’, Lingua, 130, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. S. Roberts and J. Winters (2012) ‘Social Structure and Language Structure: The New Nomothetic Approach’, Psychology of Language and Communication, 16, 89–112.Google Scholar
  55. S. Roberts and J. Winters (2013) ‘Linguistic Diversity and Traffic Accidents: Lessons from Statistical Studies of Cultural Traits’, PLOS1 August 14, 2013. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070902Google Scholar
  56. E. Santacreu-Vasut, A. Shoham and V. Gay (2013) ‘Do Female/Male Distinctions in Language Matter? Evidence from Gender Political Quotas’, Applied Economics Letters, 20, 495–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. E. Santacreu-Vasut, O. Shenkar and A. Shoham (2014) ‘Linguistic Gender Marking and its International Business Ramifications’, Journal of International Business Studies, 47, 1–9.Google Scholar
  58. G. Tabellini (2008) ‘Institutions and Culture’, Journal of the European Economic Association, 6, 255–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. A. Tversky, and D. Kahneman (1981) ‘The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice’, Science, 211, 453–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. G. Vigliocco, D. Vinson, F. Paganelli and K. Dworzynski (2005) ‘Grammatical Gender Effects on Cognition: Implication for Language Learning and Language Use’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 501–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Websites

  1. http://www.replicatedtypo.com. This is a blog on evolutionary linguistics, which has involved extensive critical discussion of correlations between social and linguistic factors, including some of the work in economics.
  2. http://wals.info. The World Atlas of Language Structures (WALS). The largest database of typological linguistic information, freely available online.
  3. http://sswl.railsplayground.net. Syntactic Structures of the World’s Languages (SSWL): Another freely available online typological database, strongly influenced by generative linguistics.
  4. http://www.ethnologue.com. A catalogue of over 7,000 languages of the world.

Copyright information

© Nigel Fabb 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nigel Fabb

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations