Advertisement

Morphogenesis and Mental Process

Part of the Cognition and Language: A Series in Psycholinguistics book series (CALS)

Abstract

Argument: Parcellation and heterochrony (neoteny) reflect the pattern and rate of a growth mechanism in morphogenesis. Structure (morphology) and function (behavior) are staged realizations of morphogenetic process. This process continues in adult cognition as the actualization of the mind-brain state. Parcellation obtains in the pruning of cells and connections in early growth, inhibition in a relatively stable morphology and constraints on context-item transforms in microgeny. Selective retardation in process (neoteny) leads to growth at earlier (juvenile) phases. This accounts for the specification of the language areas and elaboration at preliminary phases in mind, in dominance, introspection, and creativity.

Keywords

Specific Language Impairment Preliminary Phasis Language Area Double Dissociation Developmental Language Disorder 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Notes

  1. 2.
    G. Stent, “Strength and Weakness of the Genetic Approach to the Development of the Nervous System,” Annual Review of Neuroscience 4(1981):163–194.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 3.
    Brown, Self and Process, 41–45; G. Streidter and R. Northcutt, “Biological Hierarchies and the Concept of Homology,” Brain, Behavior and Evolution 38(1991):177–189.Google Scholar
  3. 4.
    K. Pribram, Brain and Perception (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1991), 25.Google Scholar
  4. 5.
    C. von de Malsburg and W. Singer, “Principles of Cortical Network Organization,” in Neurobiology of Neocortex, ed. P. Rakic, and W. Singer, (New York: Wiley, 1988). R. Thatcher, “Cyclic Cortical Reorganization: Origins of Human Cognitive Development,” in Human Behavior and the Developing Brain, ed. G. Dowson and K. Fischer (New York: Guilford Press), 232–268.Google Scholar
  5. 6.
    D. Tucker, “Developing Emotions and Cortical Networks,” in M. Gunnar, and C. Nelson (Eds.), Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology, Volume 24. Development and Behavioral Neuroscience (75–128). (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1992); J. W. Brown, Overview. In Neurobiology of Higher Functions, ed. A. Scheibel and A. Wechsler (New York: Guilford, 1990).Google Scholar
  6. 7.
    B. Goodwin, “Changing from an Evolutionary to a Generative Paradigm in Biology,” in J. Pollard (Ed.), Evolutionary Theory: Paths into the Future (New York: Wiley, 1984), 99–120.Google Scholar
  7. 8.
    M. Katz, “Ontophyletics: Studying Evolution Beyond the Genome,” Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 26(1983):323–333.Google Scholar
  8. 9.
    G. Edelman, Neural Darwinism (New York: Basic Books, 1987).Google Scholar
  9. 10.
    P. Rakic, “Competitive Interactions during Neuronal and Synaptic Development,” in From Reading to Neurons, ed. A. Galaburda (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1989); P. Rakic, “Developmental Origin of Cortical Diversity,” Schmitt lecture, Rockefeller University (1992). The basis for parcellation is unclear. There is evidence that the activity of the postsynaptic cell regulates the stability of the synapse in a retrograde manner. See J.-P. Changeux, Neuronal Man, trans. L. Garey (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1985).Google Scholar
  10. 11.
    S. Ebbeson, “Evolution and Ontogeny of Neural Circuits,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7(1984):321–366.Google Scholar
  11. 12.
    G. Innocenti, “Commentary,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7(1984):340–341.Google Scholar
  12. 13.
    S. Witelson, “Structural Correlates of Cognition in the Human Brain,” in Neurobiology of Higher Functions, ed. A. Scheibel, and A. Wechsler (New York: Guilford, 1990).Google Scholar
  13. 14.
    O. Creutzfeldt, “Generality of the Functional Structure of the Neocortex,” Naturwissenschaften 64(1977):507–517.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 15.
    Ebbeson, “Evolution and Ontogeny.”Google Scholar
  15. 16.
    G. Coghill, Anatomy and the Problem of Behavior (New York: Hafner, 1964).Google Scholar
  16. 17.
    J. Semmes, “Hemispheric Specialization: A Possible Clue to Mechanism,” Neuropsychologia 6(1968):11–26; J. W. Brown, “Lateralization: A Brain Model,” Brain and Language 5(1978):258–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 18.
    P. Wall, “Recruitment of Ineffective Synapses after Injury,” in Functional Recovery in Neurological Disease, ed. S. Waxman (New York: Raven Press, 1988).Google Scholar
  18. 19.
    A. Karmiloff-Smith, “Beyond Modularity: Innate Constraints and Developmental Change,” in The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition, ed. S. Carey and R. Gelman (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1991).Google Scholar
  19. 20.
    Pribram, Brain and Perception.Google Scholar
  20. 21.
    C. Best, “The Emergence of Native-Language Phonological Influences in Infants: A Perceptual Assimilation Model,” Haskins Laboratory Status Report on Speech Perception. SR 107–108:1–30.Google Scholar
  21. 22.
    F. Keil, “The Emergence of Theoretical Beliefs as Constraints on Concepts,” in The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition, ed. S. Carey and R. Gelman (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1991).Google Scholar
  22. 23.
    E. Perecman and J. W. Brown, “Phonemic Jargon,” in Jargonaphasia, ed. J. W. Brown (New York: Academic Press), 198, 177–258.Google Scholar
  23. 24.
    C. Trevarthen, “Emotions in Infancy,” in Approaches to Emotion, ed. K. Scherer and P. Ekman (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1984).Google Scholar
  24. 25.
    A. Diamond, “Developmental Time Course in Human Infants and Infant Monkeys, and the Neural Bases of Inhibitory Control in Reaching,” in The Development and Neural Bases of Higher Cognitive Functions, ed. A. Diamond. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 608(1990):637–69.Google Scholar
  25. 26.
    P. McNeilage, M. Studdert-Kennedy, and B. Lindblom, “Primate Handedness Reconsidered,” Behavioral Brain Sciences 10(1987):247–303.Google Scholar
  26. 27.
    G. Goldberg, “Supplementary Motor Area Structure and Function:Review and Hypotheses,” Behavioral Brain Sciences 8(1985):567–616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 28.
    L. Weiskrantz, Blindsight: A Case Study and Implications (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1986; L. Bard, “De la persistance des sensations lumineuses dans le champ aveugle des hémianopsiques,” Semaine Médicale 25(1905):253–55. M. Bender, and H. Krieger, “Visual Function in Perimetrically Blind Fields,” Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 65(1951):72–99.Google Scholar
  28. 29.
    A. Reber, “The Cognitive Unconscious: An Evolutionary Perspective, Consciousness and Cognition 1(1992).Google Scholar
  29. 30.
    R. Van Sluyters, J. Atkinson, M. Banks, R. Held, K. Hoffmann, and C. Shatz, “The Development of Vision and Visual Perception,” in Visual Perception: The Neurophysiological Foundations, ed. L. Spillman and J. Werner (New York: Academic Press, 1988).Google Scholar
  30. 31.
    Brown, “Overview” (1990).Google Scholar
  31. 32.
    B. Goodwin, “Development and Evolution, Journal of Theoretical Biology 97(1982):43–55.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 33.
    P. MacLean, “Neofrontocerebellar Evolution in Regard to Computation and Prediction:Some Fractal Aspects of Microgenesis,” in Cognitive Microgenesis: A Neuropsychological Perspective, ed. R. Hanlon (New York: Springer-Verlag, 1991); D. Robertson, “Feedback Theory and Darwinian Evolution,” Journal of Theoretical Biology 152/4(1991):469–484; L. Vandervert, “Systems Thinking and a Proposal for a Neurological Positivism, Systems Research 5(1988):313–321; 7(1990):1–17.Google Scholar
  33. 34.
    S. Gould, “Change in Developmental Timing as a Mechanism of Macroevolution,” in J. Bonner (Ed.), Evolution and Development (pp. 333–346). Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1982.Google Scholar
  34. 35.
    S. Gould, Ontogeny and Phylogeny (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1977).Google Scholar
  35. 36.
    J. Bonner and H. Horn, “Selection for Size, Shape and Developmental Timing,” in Evolution and Development, Dahlem Conference, ed. J. Bonner (Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1982), 259–276.Google Scholar
  36. 37.
    Serres (1860), cited in Gould, Ontogeny and Phylogeny.Google Scholar
  37. 38.
    J.H. Jackson, Selected Writings, 2 vols., ed. James Taylor (London: Hodder and Houghton, 1931).Google Scholar
  38. 39.
    R. Jakobsen (1968) Child Language, Aphasia, and Phonological Universals (The Hague: Mouton, 1968).Google Scholar
  39. 40.
    R. Thatcher, “Are Rhythms of Human Cerebral Development “Traveling Waves”? Behavioral Brain Sciences 14(1992a):575.Google Scholar
  40. 41.
    R. Hoffman, “Computer Simulations of Neural Information Processing and the Schizophrenia-Mania Dichotomy, Archives of General Psychiatry 44(1987):178–188.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. 42.
    B. Sahlén, From Depth to Surface: A Case Study Approach to Severe Developmental Language Disorders. Studies in Logopedics and Phoniatrics No. 1, Lund University (1991).Google Scholar
  42. 43.
    E. Bishop, “The Underlying Nature of Specific Language Impairment,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 33(1992):3–66.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. 44.
    Bishop, “Language Impairment.”Google Scholar
  44. 45.
    F. Sanides, “Comparative Neurology of the Temporal Lobe in Primates Including Man with Reference to Speech,” Brain and Language 2(1975):396–419.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. 47.
    Self and Process, 39.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1996

Personalised recommendations